Watching a cursor distorts haptically guided reproduction of mouse movement.

Participants moved a mouse along a force-feedback-defined linear path, either without vision or while watching a cursor set to 1 of 3 levels of visual:haptic gain (all >1:1). They attempted to haptically reproduce the movement without visual feedback. Errors increased with gain, reaching 70% overestimation at the highest gain. Forewarning participants about gain variability did not eliminate this effect. The gain level was potentially cued during the movement by the mismatch between visual feedback and kinesthetic feedback. Moreover, because participants did not achieve cursor-speed constancy across gain levels, visual speed was another cue to gain. Collectively, these cues failed to prevent visual distortion of movement reproduction.

[1]  M. Smyth,et al.  4 – Memory for Movements , 1984 .

[2]  S. Lederman,et al.  Perception of texture by vision and touch: multidimensionality and intersensory integration. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  E A Roy,et al.  The codability of kinesthetic location and distance information. , 1972, Acta psychologica.

[4]  Wayne A. Hershberger,et al.  Haptic estimates of discordant visual—haptic size vary developmentally , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[5]  Y. Coello,et al.  Pointing movement visually controlled through a video display: adaptation to scale change , 2000, Ergonomics.

[6]  A. Wing,et al.  The Psychology of human movement , 1984 .

[7]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[8]  G. J. Laabs Retention characteristics of different reproduction cues in motor short-term memory. , 1973, Journal of experimental psychology.

[9]  R. H. Day,et al.  Visual capture of haptically judged depth , 1969 .

[10]  A. Milewski,et al.  Strategies in cross-modality matching , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[11]  R L Klatzky,et al.  Spatial and movement-based heuristics for encoding pattern information through touch. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[12]  H. Pick,et al.  Visual capture produced by prism spectacles , 1965 .

[13]  I ROCK,et al.  Vision and Touch: An Experimentally Created Conflict between the Two Senses , 1964, Science.

[14]  J. J. Denier van der Gon,et al.  Integration of proprioceptive and visual position-information , 1999 .

[15]  Slobodan Jaric,et al.  The effects of practice on movement distance and final position reproduction: implications for the equilibrium-point control of movements , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see , 1998, Nature.

[17]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Haptic interaction with virtual objects. Spatial perception and motor control. , 2000, Biological cybernetics.

[18]  R. D. Easton,et al.  A quantitative confirmation of visual capture of curvature. , 1978, The Journal of general psychology.

[19]  I. Rock,et al.  Vision and touch. , 1967, Scientific American.

[20]  S. M. Luria,et al.  Conflicting visual and tactual-kinesthetic stimulation , 1970 .

[21]  M. Heller,et al.  Intersensory conflict between vision and touch: The response modality dominates when precise, attention-riveting judgments are required , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[22]  G. Ekman,et al.  ROUGHNESS, SMOOTHNESS, AND PREFERENCE: A STUDY OF QUANTITATIVE RELATIONS IN INDIVIDUAL SUBJECTS. , 1965, Journal of experimental psychology.

[23]  W. Helsen,et al.  A century later: Woodworth's (1899) two-component model of goal-directed aiming. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[24]  Anne E. Garing,et al.  Calibration of human locomotion and models of perceptual-motor organization. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  E. Poulton Models for biases in judging sensory magnitude. , 1979, Psychological bulletin.

[26]  S. Lederman,et al.  Texture perception: studies of intersensory organization using a discrepancy paradigm, and visual versus tactual psychophysics. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  J. Gibson,et al.  Adaptation, after-effect and contrast in the perception of curved lines. , 1933 .

[28]  Gavin P. Lawrence,et al.  Online versus offline processing of visual feedback in the control of movement amplitude. , 2003, Acta psychologica.

[29]  M. Hollins,et al.  Perception of the length of voluntary movements. , 1988, Somatosensory research.

[30]  Wayne A. Hershberger,et al.  Touch dominates haptic estimates of discordant visual-haptic size , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[31]  M. Teghtsoonian,et al.  Two varieties of perceived length , 1970 .

[32]  R. H. Day,et al.  Radial and tangential movement directions as determinants of the haptic illusion in an L figure. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.