Head and trunk movements in the frontal plane during complex dynamic equilibrium tasks in humans

Eight normal human subjects were asked to maintain monopodal equilibrium on a narrow beam (task 1) or bipodal equilibrium on an unstable rocking platform (task 2) for 5 s. Each task was performed under four experimental conditions: (1) in light, (2) in darkness, (3) in light while subject had to hold a full cup of water, and (4) as in 3, but with additional instructions to fix the gaze on the cup. The movements of the trunk and head in the frontal plane were recorded by means of a 50-Hz TV image analyzer that computed the coordinates of small reflective markers glued on the skin of the subjects. On the beam the trunk was inclined on the side of the supporting foot (13 ± 9°), on the rocking platform the mean trunk orientation during the tests was nearly vertical (2 ± 7°). Nevertheless, in both tasks the mean head position was the same and close to vertical: 1.5 ± 4° on the rocking platform and 1.5 ± 5° on the beam. For both tasks and all experimental conditions the head remained stabilized relative to vertical, despite large translations in the frontal plane. Standard deviations of head orientation from its mean value were 2.8 ± 2° for task 1 and 2 ± 1.5° for task 2. The changes of trunk orientation were significantly higher: 6.2+4.8° and 4.5 ± 4°, respectively. The differences in angular stability of head and trunk, measured through the standard deviations of angular displacements, were especially pronounced in trials with large trunk movements. It was concluded that head angular stabilization, providing the central nervous system with necessary visual and vestibular references, is essential for effective dynamic postural control in the frontal plane during complex equilibrium tasks.

[1]  J. Dichgans,et al.  The angle of visual roll motion determines displacement of subjective visual vertical , 1977 .

[2]  Hermann Aubert,et al.  Eine scheinbare bedeutende Drehung von Objecten bei Neigung des Kopfes nach rechts oder links , 1861, Archiv für pathologische Anatomie und Physiologie und für klinische Medicin.

[3]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Egocentric orientation is influenced by trained voluntary cyclorotary eye movements , 1978, Nature.

[4]  V. S. Gurfinkel,et al.  Perceptual and automatic aspects of the postural body scheme. , 1991 .

[5]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Head kinematic during various motor tasks in humans. , 1989, Progress in brain research.

[6]  T. Stoffregen Flow structure versus retinal location in the optical control of stance. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Human ocular counterroll: assessment of static and dynamic properties from electromagnetic scleral coil recordings , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  J. Droulez,et al.  Topological Aspects of Sensori-Motor Control , 1988 .

[9]  Ma Gresty,et al.  Vestibular and Visual Control on Posture and Locomotor Equilibrium , 1986 .

[10]  C. Marsden,et al.  Human postural responses. , 1981, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Head stabilization during various locomotor tasks in humans , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  D E Parker,et al.  Tilt from ahead-inverted position produces displacement of visual subjective vertical in the opposite direction , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[13]  F. Horak,et al.  Central programming of postural movements: adaptation to altered support-surface configurations. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  T. Brandt,et al.  Visual Acuity, Visual Field and Visual Scene Characteristics Affect Postural Balance1 , 1985 .

[15]  J. Massion,et al.  Anticipatory postural changes induced by active unloading and comparison with passive unloading in man , 1982, Pflügers Archiv.

[16]  A Straube,et al.  Visual stabilization of posture. Physiological stimulus characteristics and clinical aspects. , 1984, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  S. V. Fomin,et al.  On the Dynamic Equilibrium of the Pedatar Systems , 1973 .

[18]  L R Young,et al.  Influence of head orientation on visually induced pitch and roll sensation. , 1975, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[19]  N. Wade Visual orientation during and after lateral head, body, and trunk tilt , 1968 .

[20]  Elliot Saltzman,et al.  Levels of sensorimotor representation , 1979 .

[21]  R. Held,et al.  Moving Visual Scenes Influence the Apparent Direction of Gravity , 1972, Science.

[22]  H. Mittelstaedt A new solution to the problem of the subjective vertical , 1983, Naturwissenschaften.

[23]  L. Young,et al.  Integration of semicircular canal and otolith information for multisensory orientation stimuli , 1977 .

[24]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Habituation of Postural Readjustments Induced by Motion of Visual Scenes , 1985 .

[25]  Donald O. Walter,et al.  Models of the structural-functional organization of certain biological systems , 1973 .

[26]  R. J. Leigh,et al.  Frequency and velocity of rotational head perturbations during locomotion , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.