Cortical networks for reference-frame processing are shared by language and spatial navigation systems

Abstract To help us live in the three‐dimensional world, our brain integrates incoming spatial information into reference frames, which are based either on our own body (egocentric) or independent from it (allocentric). Such frames, however, may be crucial not only when interacting with the visual world, but also in language comprehension, since even the simplest utterance can be understood from different perspectives. While significant progress has been made in elucidating how linguistic factors, such as pronouns, influence reference frame adoption, the neural underpinnings of this ability are largely unknown. Building on the neural reuse framework, this study tested the hypothesis that reference frame processing in language comprehension involves mechanisms used in navigation and spatial cognition. We recorded EEG activity in 28 healthy volunteers to identify spatiotemporal dynamics in (1) spatial navigation, and (2) a language comprehension task (sentence‐picture matching). By decomposing the EEG signal into a set of maximally independent activity patterns, we localised and identified a subset of components which best characterised perspective‐taking in both domains. Remarkably, we find individual co‐variability across these tasks: people's strategies in spatial navigation are also reflected in their construction of sentential perspective. Furthermore, a distributed network of cortical generators of such strategy‐dependent activity responded not only in navigation, but in sentence comprehension. Thus we report, for the first time, evidence for shared brain mechanisms across these two domains ‐ advancing our understanding of language's interaction with other cognitive systems, and the individual differences shaping comprehension. HighlightsThe neural basis of perspective processing in language is poorly understood.We recorded EEG activity while participants comprehended and evaluated sentences.Language‐driven simulation of perspective draws upon spatial cognitive mechanisms.Individual differences in reference frame usage bias language processing and EEG.

[1]  Barbara Tversky,et al.  11 Mental models of spatial relations and transformations from language , 1999 .

[2]  F. L. D. Silva,et al.  Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[3]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Causal Influence of Articulatory Motor Cortex on Comprehending Single Spoken Words: TMS Evidence , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[4]  Phillip Wolff,et al.  Causal reasoning with forces , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[5]  N Burgess,et al.  Bilateral hippocampal pathology impairs topographical and episodic memory but not visual pattern matching , 2001, Hippocampus.

[6]  Fred W. Mast,et al.  The neural basis of the egocentric and allocentric spatial frame of reference , 2007, Brain Research.

[7]  Y. Benjamini,et al.  Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing , 1995 .

[8]  Alain Berthoz,et al.  Multiple reference frames used by the human brain for spatial perception and memory , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  Automatic perceptual simulation of first language meanings during second language sentence processing in bilinguals. , 2014, Acta psychologica.

[11]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Topographical disorientation: a synthesis and taxonomy. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  S. Raghavachari,et al.  Gating of Human Theta Oscillations by a Working Memory Task , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  D. Rosenthal,et al.  Quality-space theory in olfaction , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[14]  G. Lynch,et al.  The Control of the False Discovery Rate in Fixed Sequence Multiple Testing , 2016, 1611.03146.

[15]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Independent EEG Sources Are Dipolar , 2012, PloS one.

[16]  Nikola Vukovic,et al.  Individual differences in spatial cognition influence mental simulation of language , 2015, Cognition.

[17]  Marios N. Avraamides,et al.  The Conversational Partner's Perspective Affects Spatial Memory and Descriptions. , 2013 .

[18]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Human Body , 2001 .

[19]  Walter F. Bischof,et al.  Spatial Navigation in Virtual Reality Environments: An EEG Analysis , 2003, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[20]  Klaus Gramann,et al.  Spatial Cognition & Computation: an Interdisciplinary Journal Embodiment of Spatial Reference Frames and Individual Differences in Reference Frame Proclivity Embodiment of Spatial Reference Frames and Individual Differences in Reference Frame Proclivity , 2022 .

[21]  Michael P. Kaschak,et al.  Perception of motion affects language processing , 2005, Cognition.

[22]  J. Perner,et al.  Left inferior-parietal lobe activity in perspective tasks: identity statements , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[23]  Sarah S. Chance,et al.  Spatial Updating of Self-Position and Orientation During Real, Imagined, and Virtual Locomotion , 1998 .

[24]  Christoph M. Michel,et al.  Towards the utilization of EEG as a brain imaging tool , 2012, NeuroImage.

[25]  Hermann J Müller,et al.  Evidence of separable spatial representations in a virtual navigation task. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  H. Mayberg Brain Activation , 1994, Neurology.

[27]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Reading cinnamon activates olfactory brain regions , 2006, NeuroImage.

[28]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[29]  O. Blanke,et al.  Neural Basis of Embodiment: Distinct Contributions of Temporoparietal Junction and Extrastriate Body Area , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  Y. Benjamini,et al.  THE CONTROL OF THE FALSE DISCOVERY RATE IN MULTIPLE TESTING UNDER DEPENDENCY , 2001 .

[31]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Language Comprehenders Mentally Represent the Shapes of Objects , 2002, Psychological science.

[32]  B. Tversky,et al.  Perspective in Spatial Descriptions , 1996 .

[33]  Y. Shtyrov,et al.  Automatic ultrarapid activation and inhibition of cortical motor systems in spoken word comprehension , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[34]  S. Huettel,et al.  Males and females use different distal cues in a virtual environment navigation task. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[35]  D. Proffitt,et al.  Spatial updating of virtual displays during self- and display rotation. , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[36]  M. Masson,et al.  Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[37]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Revisiting Mental Simulation in Language Comprehension: Six Replication Attempts , 2012, PloS one.

[38]  Marta Kutas,et al.  Comprehending how visual context influences incremental sentence processing: Insights from ERPs and picture-sentence verification. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[39]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Dissociation of object and spatial processing domains in primate prefrontal cortex. , 1993, Science.

[40]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article THE EFFECT OF IMPLIED ORIENTATION DERIVED FROM VERBAL CONTEXT ON PICTURE RECOGNITION , 2022 .

[41]  Eleanor A. Maguire,et al.  Thoughts, behaviour, and brain dynamics during navigation in the real world , 2006, NeuroImage.

[42]  Nigel MacLennan,et al.  Assessment and Analysis , 2017 .

[43]  Marta Kutas,et al.  Postlexical integration processes during language comprehension: Evidence from brain-imaging research , 2000 .

[44]  E. Maguire,et al.  A Temporoparietal and Prefrontal Network for Retrieving the Spatial Context of Lifelike Events , 2001, NeuroImage.

[45]  Nikola Vukovic,et al.  Cortical motor systems are involved in second-language comprehension: Evidence from rapid mu-rhythm desynchronisation , 2014, NeuroImage.

[46]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Extrastriate body area in human occipital cortex responds to the performance of motor actions , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[47]  Giuseppe Iaria,et al.  Gray Matter Differences Correlate with Spontaneous Strategies in a Human Virtual Navigation Task , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  Ludovica Labruna,et al.  A Functional Role for the Motor System in Language Understanding , 2011, Psychological science.

[49]  P. Nunez,et al.  Spatial‐temporal structures of human alpha rhythms: Theory, microcurrent sources, multiscale measurements, and global binding of local networks , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[50]  S. Raghavachari,et al.  Distinct patterns of brain oscillations underlie two basic parameters of human maze learning. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[51]  G. Buzsáki Theta rhythm of navigation: Link between path integration and landmark navigation, episodic and semantic memory , 2005, Hippocampus.

[52]  Ehren L. Newman,et al.  Human θ Oscillations Related to Sensorimotor Integration and Spatial Learning , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  J. Pineda The functional significance of mu rhythms: Translating “seeing” and “hearing” into “doing” , 2005, Brain Research Reviews.

[54]  B. Bergen,et al.  The case of the missing pronouns: Does mentally simulated perspective play a functional role in the comprehension of person? , 2013, Cognition.

[55]  Michael L. Anderson Neural reuse: A fundamental organizational principle of the brain , 2010, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[56]  Nick F. Ramsey,et al.  Interactions between ego- and allocentric neuronal representations of space , 2006, NeuroImage.

[57]  Roberta L. Klatzky,et al.  Allocentric and Egocentric Spatial Representations: Definitions, Distinctions, and Interconnections , 1998, Spatial Cognition.

[58]  William S Horton,et al.  Out of sight, out of mind: Occlusion and the accessibility of information in narrative comprehension , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[59]  E. Maguire The retrosplenial contribution to human navigation: a review of lesion and neuroimaging findings. , 2001, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[60]  Denis Cousineau,et al.  Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson's method , 2005 .

[61]  L. Connell Representing object colour in language comprehension , 2007, Cognition.

[62]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Reference Frames for Spatial Cognition: Different Brain Areas are Involved in Viewer-, Object-, and Landmark-Centered Judgments About Object Location , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[63]  J. Douglas Crawford,et al.  Allocentric versus Egocentric Representation of Remembered Reach Targets in Human Cortex , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[64]  Herbert H. Clark,et al.  On the process of comparing sentences against pictures , 1972 .

[65]  Timothy P. McNamara,et al.  Systems of Spatial Reference in Human Memory , 2001, Cognitive Psychology.

[66]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Removing electroencephalographic artifacts by blind source separation. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[67]  Robert Oostenveld,et al.  The five percent electrode system for high-resolution EEG and ERP measurements , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[68]  Scott Makeig,et al.  ERP Features and EEG Dynamics : An ICA Perspective , 2009 .

[69]  Jonathan R. Whitlock,et al.  Navigating from hippocampus to parietal cortex , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[70]  F. Pulvermüller,et al.  Reading salt activates gustatory brain regions: fMRI evidence for semantic grounding in a novel sensory modality. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[71]  Flavia Filimon,et al.  Are All Spatial Reference Frames Egocentric? Reinterpreting Evidence for Allocentric, Object-Centered, or World-Centered Reference Frames , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[72]  C. Lawton Gender differences in way-finding strategies: Relationship to spatial ability and spatial anxiety , 1994 .

[73]  The case of M , 2012, BMJ.

[74]  H. Bülthoff,et al.  Spatial updating in virtual reality: the sufficiency of visual information , 2007, Psychological research.

[75]  Hermann J. Müller,et al.  Human Brain Dynamics Accompanying Use of Egocentric and Allocentric Reference Frames during Navigation , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[76]  I. Johnsrude,et al.  Somatotopic Representation of Action Words in Human Motor and Premotor Cortex , 2004, Neuron.

[77]  Martin J. Pickering,et al.  Perspective taking in language: integrating the spatial and action domains , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[78]  Marc Jeannerod,et al.  The Agent is Right: When Motor Embodied Cognition is Space-Dependent , 2011, PloS one.

[79]  B. Bahrami,et al.  Coming of age: A review of embodiment and the neuroscience of semantics , 2012, Cortex.

[80]  Michael P. Kaschak,et al.  Grounding language in action , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[81]  A. Wunderlich,et al.  Brain activation during human navigation: gender-different neural networks as substrate of performance , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[82]  Marios N. Avraamides,et al.  Social and Representational Cues Jointly Influence Spatial Perspective-Taking , 2015, Cogn. Sci..

[83]  L. Barsalou Grounded cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[84]  Hermann J. Müller,et al.  Human EEG Correlates of Spatial Navigation within Egocentric and Allocentric Reference Frames , 2010, Spatial Cognition.

[85]  Yale E. Cohen,et al.  A common reference frame for movement plans in the posterior parietal cortex , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[86]  Sarah H. Creem-Regehr,et al.  Spatial updating of virtual displays , 2004 .

[87]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Validating the boundary element method for forward and inverse EEG computations in the presence of a hole in the skull , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[88]  M. Kiefer,et al.  The Sound of Concepts: Four Markers for a Link between Auditory and Conceptual Brain Systems , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[89]  S. Lacey,et al.  Engagement of the left extrastriate body area during body-part metaphor comprehension , 2017, Brain and Language.

[90]  Scott Makeig,et al.  Information-based modeling of event-related brain dynamics. , 2006, Progress in brain research.

[91]  Claudia Gianelli,et al.  Acting in perspective: the role of body and language as social tools , 2013, Psychological research.

[92]  Riccardo Dalla Volta,et al.  Does listening to action-related sentences modulate the activity of the motor system? Replication of a combined TMS and behavioral study , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[93]  B. Schönebeck,et al.  The neural basis of ego- and allocentric reference frames in spatial navigation: Evidence from spatio-temporal coupled current density reconstruction , 2006, Brain Research.

[94]  S Makeig,et al.  Blind separation of auditory event-related brain responses into independent components. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[95]  C. Colby Action-Oriented Spatial Reference Frames in Cortex , 1998, Neuron.

[96]  M. Petrides,et al.  Cognitive Strategies Dependent on the Hippocampus and Caudate Nucleus in Human Navigation: Variability and Change with Practice , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[97]  Tzyy-Ping Jung,et al.  Alpha modulation in parietal and retrosplenial cortex correlates with navigation performance. , 2012, Psychophysiology.

[98]  J. Sanes,et al.  Spatial coding of visual and somatic sensory information in body‐centred coordinates , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[99]  B. Schönebeck,et al.  Die Tunnelaufgabe: Eine Methode zur Untersuchung kognitiver Teilprozesse räumlicher Orientierungsleistungen , 2001 .

[100]  Michael Burke,et al.  Taking Perspective: Personal Pronouns Affect Experiential Aspects of Literary Reading , 2016, PloS one.

[101]  Steven Laureys,et al.  Cytology and functionally correlated circuits of human posterior cingulate areas , 2006, NeuroImage.

[102]  Cheryl L. Grady,et al.  Dissociation of object and spatial visual processing pathways in , 1993 .

[103]  Weimin Mou,et al.  Allocentric and egocentric updating of spatial memories. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[104]  A. Berthoz,et al.  The neural basis of egocentric and allocentric coding of space in humans: a functional magnetic resonance study , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[105]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[106]  L. Barsalou,et al.  Grounding the human conceptual system in perception, action, and internal states , 2013 .

[107]  T. Matlock Fictive motion as cognitive simulation , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[108]  C. Büchel,et al.  Differential Recruitment of the Hippocampus, Medial Prefrontal Cortex, and the Human Motion Complex during Path Integration in Humans , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[109]  Nikola Vukovic,et al.  Primary motor cortex functionally contributes to language comprehension: An online rTMS study , 2017, Neuropsychologia.

[110]  Chin-Teng Lin,et al.  EEG correlates of spatial orientation in the human retrosplenial complex , 2015, NeuroImage.

[111]  Alain Berthoz,et al.  A fronto-parietal system for computing the egocentric spatial frame of reference in humans , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[112]  Peter König,et al.  Different strategies for spatial updating in yaw and pitch path integration , 2013, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[113]  Tad T. Brunyé,et al.  When You and I Share Perspectives , 2009, Psychological science.

[114]  S. Makeig Auditory event-related dynamics of the EEG spectrum and effects of exposure to tones. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[115]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Short article: Language comprehenders retain implied shape and orientation of objects , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[116]  Nicole Etchamendy,et al.  Spontaneous navigational strategies and performance in the virtual town , 2007, Hippocampus.