Top-down deactivation of interference from irrelevant spatial or verbal stimulus features

The selective-attention model of Houghton and Tipper (1994) assumes top-down deactivation of (conflicting) distractor representations as a mechanism of visual attention. Deactivation should produce an inverted-U-shaped activation function for distractor representations. In a recent study, Frings, Wentura, and Wühr (2012) tested this prediction in a variant of the flanker task in which a cue sometimes required participants to respond to the distractors rather than to the target. When reaction times and error rates were plotted as a function of the target–cue stimulus onset asynchrony, a quadratic trend emerged, consistent with the notion of distractor deactivation. However, in the flanker task, an alternative explanation for the quadratic trend in terms of attentional zooming is possible. The present experiments tested the deactivation account against the attentional-zooming account with the Stroop and the Simon task, in which attentional zooming should have minimal effects on distractor processing, because the target and distractor are presented at the same spatial location. Both experiments replicated the quadratic trend in the performance functions for responses to incongruent distractors, and additionally showed linear trends in the performance functions for responses to congruent distractors. These results provide additional support for the notion of top-down deactivation of distractor representations as a mechanism of visual selective attention.

[1]  Thomas H. Carr,et al.  Inhibitory processes in perceptual recognition: Evidence for a Center-Surround Attentional Mechanism , 1994 .

[2]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[3]  D. Besner,et al.  What kind of attention modulates the Stroop effect? , 1999, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[4]  Negative priming is stronger for task-relevant dimensions: Evidence of flexibility in the selective ignoring of distractor information , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[5]  S. Tipper Does Negative Priming Reflect Inhibitory Mechanisms? A Review and Integration of Conflicting Views , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[6]  Dirk Wentura,et al.  Evaluative priming of naming and semantic categorization responses revisited: a mutual facilitation explanation. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[7]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .

[8]  Michael D. Dodd,et al.  In Opposition to Inhibition , 2003 .

[9]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[10]  C. Spence,et al.  Increased perceptual and conceptual processing difficulty makes the immeasurable measurable: negative priming in the absence of probe distractors. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  B. Hommel The Simon effect as tool and heuristic. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[12]  D. Wentura,et al.  The “meddling-in” of affective information: A general model of automatic evaluation effects , 2003 .

[13]  Christian Frings,et al.  On the fate of distractor representations. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  H. Heuer,et al.  Perspectives on Perception and Action , 1989 .

[15]  D. Wentura,et al.  Response-bound primes diminish affective priming in the naming task , 2008 .

[16]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Interference and Facilitation Effects during Selective Attention: An H2 15O PET Study of Stroop Task Performance , 1995, NeuroImage.

[17]  Colin M. Macleod,et al.  Inhibition in Cognition , 2007 .

[18]  K. C. Klauer,et al.  The Psychology of Evaluation : Affective Processes in Cognition and Emotion , 2003 .

[19]  S. Tipper,et al.  Inhibitory Mechanisms of Neural and Cognitive Control: Applications to Selective Attention and Sequential Action , 1996, Brain and Cognition.

[20]  J. Nigg On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[21]  K Richard Ridderinkhof,et al.  Delta plots in the study of individual differences: new tools reveal response inhibition deficits in AD/Hd that are eliminated by methylphenidate treatment. , 2005, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[22]  Robert W. Proctor,et al.  Stimulus-Response Compatibility: An Integrated Perspective , 1990 .

[23]  John W. Tukey,et al.  Exploratory Data Analysis. , 1979 .

[24]  Andrea M Philipp,et al.  Control and interference in task switching--a review. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[25]  S. Tipper,et al.  A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. , 1994 .

[26]  James L. McClelland,et al.  On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. , 1990, Psychological review.

[27]  W. Trammell Neill,et al.  Episodic retrieval in negative priming and repetition priming. , 1997 .

[28]  P. Wühr,et al.  A case for inhibition: visual attention suppresses the processing of irrelevant objects. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[29]  Shaffer Lh CHOICE REACTION WITH VARIABLE S-R MAPPING. , 1965 .

[30]  J. Danckert Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action: Attention and Performance XIX Wolfgang Prinz, Bernhard Hommel (Eds.), Oxford University Press, 2002, Price: £ 65.00, ISBN: 0-19-851069 , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  R. Proctor,et al.  The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[32]  T. Carr,et al.  Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory and Language , 1994 .

[33]  A. Treisman Contextual Cues in Selective Listening , 1960 .

[34]  M. Posner The Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention , 2020 .

[35]  A. Henik,et al.  Evidence for task conflict in the Stroop effect. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[36]  Colin M. Macleod,et al.  Interdimensional interference in the Stroop effect: uncovering the cognitive and neural anatomy of attention , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[37]  D. LaBerge Spatial extent of attention to letters and words. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[38]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Activation and suppression in conflict tasks: Empirical clarification through distributional analyses. , 2002 .

[39]  S. Tipper The Negative Priming Effect: Inhibitory Priming by Ignored Objects , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[40]  Shaffer Lh,et al.  Some effects of partial advance information on choice reaction with fixed or variable S-R mapping. , 1966 .

[41]  R. H. Phaf,et al.  SLAM: A connectionist model for attention in visual selection tasks , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.

[42]  R. Proctor,et al.  Stimulus-Response Compatibility Principles: Data, Theory, and Application , 2006 .

[43]  Cathleen M. Moore,et al.  Negative priming depends on probe-trial conflict: Where has all the inhibition gone? , 1994 .

[44]  O. Neumann Beyond capacity: A functional view of attention , 1987 .

[45]  J. R. Simon The Effects of an Irrelevant Directional CUE on Human Information Processing , 1990 .

[46]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Response inhibition in conflict tasks is revealed in delta plots , 2004 .

[47]  A. Buchner,et al.  Negative Priming as a Memory Phenomenon , 2007 .

[48]  H. Pashler The Psychology of Attention , 1997 .

[49]  J. Michael Herrmann,et al.  A computational approach to negative priming , 2007, Connect. Sci..

[50]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.