Children’s first and second-order false-belief reasoning in a verbal and a low-verbal task

We can understand and act upon the beliefs of other people, even when these conflict with our own beliefs. Children’s development of this ability, known as Theory of Mind, typically happens around age 4. Research using a looking-time paradigm, however, established that toddlers at the age of 15 months old pass a non-verbal false-belief task (Onishi and Baillargeon in Science 308:255–258, 2005). This is well before the age at which children pass any of the verbal false-belief tasks. In this study we present a more complex case of false-belief reasoning with older children. We tested second-order reasoning, probing children’s ability to handle the belief of one person about the belief of another person. We find just the opposite: 7-year-olds pass a verbal false-belief reasoning task, but fail on an equally complex low-verbal task. This finding suggests that language supports explicit reasoning about beliefs, perhaps by facilitating the cognitive system to keep track of beliefs attributed by people to other people.

[1]  L. J. Friedman Child Development , 1971, Nature.

[2]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception , 1983, Cognition.

[3]  H. Wimmer,et al.  “John thinks that Mary thinks that…” attribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old children ☆ , 1985 .

[4]  Henry M. Wellman,et al.  Young children's reasoning about beliefs , 1988, Cognition.

[5]  J. Astington,et al.  The child''s discovery of mind , 1994 .

[6]  Helen Tager-Flusberg,et al.  Preschoolers can attribute second-order beliefs , 1994 .

[7]  T. Hedden,et al.  What do you think I think you think?: Strategic reasoning in matrix games , 2002, Cognition.

[8]  T. Ruffman,et al.  The relation between children's and mothers' mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. , 2002, Child development.

[9]  J. Dunn,et al.  Communication, Relationships, and Individual Differences in Children's Understanding of Mind. , 2005 .

[10]  Sunny Shin,et al.  Do 15-Month-Old Infants Understand False Beliefs ? , 2005 .

[11]  J. Perner,et al.  Infants' Insight into the Mind: How Deep? , 2005, Science.

[12]  Jodie A. Baird,et al.  Why Language Matters for Theory of Mind , 2005 .

[13]  de Villiers,et al.  Can Language Acquisition Give Children a Point of View , 2005 .

[14]  J. Astington,et al.  Language and theory of mind: meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. , 2007, Child development.

[15]  T. Roeper The prism of grammar : how child language illuminates humanism , 2007 .

[16]  Thomas Roeper The prism of grammar , 2007 .

[17]  Anna Gavarró,et al.  Language Acquisition and Development: Proceedings of GALA 2007 , 2008 .

[18]  Rineke Verbrugge,et al.  Children’s Application of Theory of Mind in Reasoning and Language , 2008, J. Log. Lang. Inf..

[19]  Lawrence W. Barsalou,et al.  Perceptual Processing Affects Conceptual Processing , 2008, Cogn. Sci..

[20]  Thomas Roeper,et al.  Second order embedding and second order false belief , 2008 .

[21]  Ian A Apperly,et al.  Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? , 2009, Psychological review.

[22]  R. Catrambone,et al.  Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society , 2010 .

[23]  H. Rijn,et al.  The Facilitative Effect of Context on Second-Order Social Reasoning , 2010 .

[24]  S. L. Ornat,et al.  Language Acquisition and Development , 2012 .

[25]  L. C. De Bruin,et al.  The developmental paradox of false belief understanding: a dual-system solution , 2012, Synthese.

[26]  Joel A. C. Baum,et al.  What Do You Think I Think You Think about It , 2014 .