Longitudinal evidence for diminished frontal cortex function in aging

Cross-sectional estimates of age-related changes in brain structure and function were compared with 6-y longitudinal estimates. The results indicated increased sensitivity of the longitudinal approach as well as qualitative differences. Critically, the cross-sectional analyses were suggestive of age-related frontal overrecruitment, whereas the longitudinal analyses revealed frontal underrecruitment with advancing age. The cross-sectional observation of overrecruitment reflected a select elderly sample. However, when followed over time, this sample showed reduced frontal recruitment. These findings dispute inferences of true age changes on the basis of age differences, hence challenging some contemporary models of neurocognitive aging, and demonstrate age-related decline in frontal brain volume as well as functional response.

[1]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[2]  K. Schaie The course of adult intellectual development. , 1994, The American psychologist.

[3]  Paul B. Baltes,et al.  Intellectual Development in Adulthood: The Seattle Longitudinal Study , 1996 .

[4]  T. Salthouse,et al.  Meta-analyses of age-cognition relations in adulthood: estimates of linear and nonlinear age effects and structural models. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[5]  L. Nyberg,et al.  The betula prospective cohort study: Memory, health, and aging , 1997 .

[6]  U. Staudinger,et al.  Lifespan psychology: theory and application to intellectual functioning. , 1999, Annual review of psychology.

[7]  D. Schacter,et al.  Task-specific repetition priming in left inferior prefrontal cortex. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[8]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Isolating the neural mechanisms of age-related changes in human working memory , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Cerebral Asymmetry and the Effects of Sex and Handedness on Brain Structure: A Voxel-Based Morphometric Analysis of 465 Normal Adult Human Brains , 2001, NeuroImage.

[10]  Roberto Cabeza,et al.  Aging Gracefully: Compensatory Brain Activity in High-Performing Older Adults , 2002, NeuroImage.

[11]  J. Jolles,et al.  Predictors of attrition in a longitudinal cognitive aging study: the Maastricht Aging Study (MAAS). , 2002, Journal of clinical epidemiology.

[12]  N. Cohen,et al.  Attentional Control in the Aging Brain: Insights from an fMRI Study of the Stroop Task , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[13]  R. Cabeza Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model. , 2002, Psychology and aging.

[14]  J. Logan,et al.  Under-Recruitment and Nonselective Recruitment Dissociable Neural Mechanisms Associated with Aging , 2002, Neuron.

[15]  L. Nyberg,et al.  Betula: A Prospective Cohort Study on Memory, Health and Aging , 2004 .

[16]  D. Head,et al.  Differential vulnerability of anterior white matter in nondemented aging with minimal acceleration in dementia of the Alzheimer type: evidence from diffusion tensor imaging. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  Karsten Specht,et al.  Brain localization of attentional control in different age groups by combining functional and structural MRI , 2004, NeuroImage.

[18]  L. Nyberg,et al.  Stability, growth, and decline in adult life span development of declarative memory: cross-sectional and longitudinal data from a population-based study. , 2005, Psychology and aging.

[19]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  Region-specific changes in prefrontal function with age: a review of PET and fMRI studies on working and episodic memory. , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  Cheryl L. Dahle,et al.  Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: general trends, individual differences and modifiers. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[21]  Jonas Persson,et al.  Structure-function correlates of cognitive decline in aging. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[22]  J. Pekar,et al.  Method for multimodal analysis of independent source differences in schizophrenia: Combining gray matter structural and auditory oddball functional data , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[23]  A. Pfefferbaum,et al.  Diffusion tensor imaging and aging , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[24]  Jonas Persson,et al.  Reduced functional brain activity response in cognitively intact apolipoprotein E epsilon4 carriers. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  Jonas Persson,et al.  Reduced functional brain activity response in cognitively intact apolipoprotein E ε4 carriers , 2006 .

[26]  L. Nyberg,et al.  Altered brain activity in healthy seniors: what does it mean? , 2006, Progress in brain research.

[27]  L. Nyberg,et al.  The correlative triad among aging, dopamine, and cognition: Current status and future prospects , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[28]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Que PASA? The posterior-anterior shift in aging. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[29]  Denise C. Park,et al.  The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. , 2009, Annual review of psychology.

[30]  A. Dale,et al.  One-Year Brain Atrophy Evident in Healthy Aging , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  Hauke R. Heekeren,et al.  Performance level modulates adult age differences in brain activation during spatial working memory , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[32]  R. N. Spreng,et al.  Reliable differences in brain activity between young and old adults: A quantitative meta-analysis across multiple cognitive domains , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.