Our eyes deviate away from a location where a distractor is expected to appear

Previous research has shown that in order to make an accurate saccade to a target object, nearby distractor objects need to be inhibited. The extent to which saccade trajectories deviate away from a distractor is often considered to be an index of the strength of inhibition. The present study shows that the mere expectation that a distractor will appear at a specific location is enough to generate saccade deviations away from this location. This suggests that higher-order cognitive processes such as top-down expectancy interact with low-level structures involved in eye movement control. The results will be discussed in the light of current theories of target selection and possible neurophysiological correlates.

[1]  W. C. Hall,et al.  The Superior Colliculus : New Approaches for Studying Sensorimotor Integration , 2003 .

[2]  D. Meyer,et al.  Attention and Performance XIV , 1973 .

[3]  W. Becker,et al.  An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli , 1979, Vision Research.

[4]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Remembering a Location Makes the Eyes Curve Away , 2005, Psychological science.

[5]  R. Walker,et al.  Curved saccade trajectories: Voluntary and reflexive saccades curve away from irrelevant distractors , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  A J Van Opstal,et al.  Blink-perturbed saccades in monkey. II. Superior colliculus activity. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  R. Wurtz,et al.  The Neurobiology of Saccadic Eye Movements , 1989 .

[9]  S. Tipper,et al.  A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. , 1994 .

[10]  T. Carr,et al.  Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory and Language , 1994 .

[11]  M. Frens,et al.  Spatial and temporal factors determine auditory-visual interactions in human saccadic eye movements , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[12]  Casimir J. H. Ludwig,et al.  Measuring saccade curvature: A curve-fitting approach , 2002, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[13]  Christian Quaia,et al.  The maintenance of spatial accuracy by the perisaccadic remapping of visual receptive fields , 1998, Neural Networks.

[14]  D. Munoz Commentary: saccadic eye movements: overview of neural circuitry. , 2002, Progress in brain research.

[15]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  The relationship between inhibition of return and saccade trajectory deviations. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  Eugene McSorley,et al.  Distractor modulation of saccade trajectories: spatial separation and symmetry effects , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  Casper J. Erkelens,et al.  Initial directions and landing positions of binocular saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[18]  A. J. Van Opstal,et al.  Three-dimensional analysis of strongly curved saccades elicited by double-step stimuli , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Inhibition-of-return and oculomotor interference , 2004, Vision Research.

[20]  A. Georgopoulos Current issues in directional motor control , 1995, Trends in Neurosciences.

[21]  Casimir J. H. Ludwig,et al.  Target similarity affects saccade curvature away from irrelevant onsets , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  M. Schlag-Rey,et al.  How the frontal eye field can impose a saccade goal on superior colliculus neurons. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Spatial attention and eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  Jeffrey D. Schall,et al.  CONCURRENT, DISTRIBUTED CONTROL OF SACCADE INITIATION IN THE FRONTAL EYE FIELD AND SUPERIOR COLLICULUS , 2003 .

[25]  A. Opstal,et al.  STIMULUS-INDUCED MIDFLIGHT MODIFICATION OF SACCADE TRAJECTORIES , 1987 .

[26]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Programming of endogenous and exogenous saccades: evidence for a competitive integration model. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  Daniel Guitton,et al.  Superior colliculus encodes distance to target, not saccade amplitude, in multi-step gaze shifts , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[28]  A. Leventhal The neural basis of visual function , 1991 .

[29]  P. Viviani,et al.  The curvature of oblique saccades , 1977, Vision Research.

[30]  S. Tipper,et al.  Selective Attention and Priming: Inhibitory and Facilitatory Effects of Ignored Primes , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[31]  D. Sparks,et al.  The deep layers of the superior colliculus. , 1989, Reviews of oculomotor research.

[32]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  The influence of attending to multiple locations on eye movements , 2005, Vision Research.

[33]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Oculomotor capture and Inhibition of Return: Evidence for an oculomotor suppression account of IOR , 2002, Psychological research.

[34]  Adonis K Moschovakis,et al.  The superior colliculus and eye movement control , 1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[35]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Priming in Macaque Frontal Cortex during Popout Visual Search: Feature-Based Facilitation and Location-Based Inhibition of Return , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  R. Klein,et al.  A Model of Saccade Initiation Based on the Competitive Integration of Exogenous and Endogenous Signals in the Superior Colliculus , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[37]  D. Munoz,et al.  Lateral inhibitory interactions in the intermediate layers of the monkey superior colliculus. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  J. Stern Theoretical and applied aspects of eye movement research A. G. Gale and F. Johnson, (Elsevier Science Publishers B.V., Amsterdam, 1984) pp. xiii + 565, Dfl. 185 , 1985, Biological Psychology.

[39]  K. Kopecz,et al.  Saccadic reaction times in gap/overlap paradigms: a model based on integration of intentional and visual information on neural, dynamic fields , 1995, Vision Research.

[40]  Ariane Levy Schoen Détermination et latence de la réponse oculomotrice à deux stimulus simultanés ou successifs selon leur excentricité relative , 1969 .

[41]  Robert M McPeek,et al.  Competition between saccade goals in the superior colliculus produces saccade curvature. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  Hidehiko Komatsu,et al.  Projections from the functional subdivisions of the frontal eye field to the superior colliculus in the monkey , 1985, Brain Research.

[43]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Space and selective attention , 1994 .

[44]  Laurence R. Harris,et al.  Small Saccades to Double-Stepped Targets Moving in Two Dimensions , 1984 .

[45]  S. Tipper,et al.  Selective Reaching to Grasp: Evidence for Distractor Interference Effects , 1997 .

[46]  Michele A. Basso,et al.  Modulation of neuronal activity by target uncertainty , 1997, Nature.

[47]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Orienting of attention and eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[48]  A. Lévy-schoen Détermination et latence de la réponse oculomotrice à deux stimulus simultanés ou successifs selon leur excentricité relative , 1969 .

[49]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  Deficits in saccade target selection after inactivation of superior colliculus , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[50]  S. Grossberg,et al.  A neural model of saccadic eye movement control explains task-specific adaptation , 1999, Vision Research.

[51]  M. A. Basso,et al.  Neuronal Activity in Substantia Nigra Pars Reticulata during Target Selection , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Our Eyes do Not Always Go Where we Want Them to Go: Capture of the Eyes by New Objects , 1998 .