Production of time intervals from segmented and nonsegmented inputs

One important factor influencing the accuracy of a timing estimate is the counting activity that a human subject may adopt. In the present study, the usefulness of this activity is evaluated with a strategy whereby subjects are presented segmented and nonsegmented intervals, before they start to produce a series of these intervals, using a finger-tapping procedure. The results are mainly analyzed in the light of Killeen and Weiss’s (1987) model, which addressed this question of counting. The results revealed that (1) a scalar property gives a better description of the pacemaker activity than does a Poisson process, and (2) an optimal timing performance would be reached with the utilization of subintervals with an approximate val of of 400 msec. Finally, the discussion also incorporates an analysis of the variability related to the motor component in a tapping task.

[1]  S. Keele,et al.  Timing Functions of The Cerebellum , 1989, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[2]  M. Treisman,et al.  The Internal Clock: Evidence for a Temporal Oscillator Underlying Time Perception with Some Estimates of its Characteristic Frequency , 1990, Perception.

[3]  A B Kristofferson,et al.  A quantal step function in duration discrimination , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  A. R. Gilliland,et al.  Some factors in estimating short time intervals , 1940 .

[5]  Christopher Miall,et al.  The Storage of Time Intervals Using Oscillating Neurons , 1989, Neural Computation.

[6]  Robert Rousseau,et al.  An Adaptive Counter Model for Time Estimation , 1984 .

[7]  Alfred B. Kristofferson A real-time criterion theory of duration discrimination , 1977 .

[8]  R. E. Hicks,et al.  Counting eliminates the repetition effect in judgments of temporal duration. , 1979, Acta psychologica.

[9]  P. Killeen,et al.  Optimal timing and the Weber function. , 1987, Psychological review.

[10]  David J. Getty,et al.  Discrimination of short temporal intervals: A comparison of two models , 1975 .

[11]  R. Church,et al.  Alternative representations of time, number, and rate , 1990, Cognition.

[12]  Peter R. Killeen,et al.  Counting the Minutes , 1992 .

[13]  J G Fetterman,et al.  Time discrimination in Columba livia and Homo sapiens. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[14]  Warren H. Meck,et al.  Modality-specific circadian rhythmicities influence mechanisms of attention and memory for interval timing , 1991 .

[15]  William M. Petrusic,et al.  Explicit Counting and Time‐Order Errors in Duration Discrimination , 1984 .

[16]  J. Gibbon Origins of scalar timing , 1991 .

[17]  David J. Getty,et al.  Counting processes in human timing , 1976 .

[18]  J. Wearden Human Performance on an Analogue of an Interval Bisection Task , 1991, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[19]  P. Killeen,et al.  A behavioral theory of timing , 1988 .

[20]  Peter R. Killeen,et al.  A componential analysis of pacemaker-counter timing systems. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  A. Kristofferson,et al.  Response delays and the timing of discrete motor responses , 1973 .

[22]  John H. Wearden,et al.  Do humans possess an internal clock with scalar timing properties , 1991 .

[23]  C. Douglas Creelman,et al.  Human Discrimination of Auditory Duration , 1962 .

[24]  M. Treisman Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval. Implications for a model of the "internal clock". , 1963, Psychological monographs.

[25]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Human bisection at the geometric mean , 1991 .

[26]  Peter R. Killeen,et al.  Adjusting the pacemaker , 1991 .