The effects of preclinical dementia on estimates of normal cognitive functioning in aging.

Individuals with preclinical dementia have begun to decline cognitively, but still perform within normal limits on cognitive testing. As a group, subjects with preclinical dementia have lower scores on neuropsychological tests than their dementia-free counterparts. This study examines the effects of preclinical dementia on estimates of normal cognitive function in the aged using data from a longitudinal study. Individuals with preclinical dementia at baseline were retrospectively identified based on subsequent development of dementia. Age-adjusted norms were computed using baseline data for the Selective Reminding Test and the WAIS verbal and performance scores, both including (conventional norms) and then excluding (robust norms) preclinical cases. The results indicate that by failing to exclude preclinical dementia, conventional normative studies underestimate the mean, overestimate the variance, and overestimate the effect of age on cognitive measures. Methods are discussed for selecting robust elderly samples that are relatively free of contamination by preclinical dementia.

[1]  A. Monsch,et al.  Preclinical cognitive markers of dementia of the Alzheimer type. , 1994 .

[2]  M. Roth,et al.  The Association Between Quantitative Measures of Dementia and of Senile Change in the Cerebral Grey Matter of Elderly Subjects , 1968, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[3]  H. Buschke,et al.  Aging, encoding specificity, and memory change in the Double Memory Test , 1995, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[4]  H. Crystal,et al.  Predicting development of dementia in the elderly with the Selective Reminding Test. , 1990, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[5]  R. Michels,et al.  Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd ed , 1981 .

[6]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[7]  Herman Buschke,et al.  Selective reminding for analysis of memory and learning , 1973 .

[8]  H S Levin,et al.  Distinguishing normal and demented elderly with the selective reminding test. , 1989, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[9]  H. Levin,et al.  Selective reminding test: an examination of the equivalence of four forms. , 1985, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[10]  L. Jarvik,et al.  Cognitive Function and Prediction of Dementia in Old Age , 1987, International journal of aging & human development.

[11]  R. Katzman.,et al.  Education and the prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer's disease , 1993, Neurology.

[12]  R. Friedland Alzheimer's disease : clinical features and differential diagnosis , 1993 .

[13]  K. Marder,et al.  Neuropsychological detection and characterization of preclinical Alzheimer's disease , 1995, Neurology.

[14]  J. Price,et al.  Cerebral amyloid deposition and diffuse plaques in ``normal'' aging , 1996, Neurology.

[15]  E. Tangalos,et al.  Mayo's Older Americans Normative Studies: Utility of corrections for age and education for the WAIS-R , 1992 .

[16]  James F. Malec,et al.  Mayo's older americans normative studies: Updated AVLT norms for ages 56 to 97 , 1992 .

[17]  James F. Malec,et al.  Mayo's older americans normative studies: WMS-R norms for ages 56 to 94 , 1992 .

[18]  M. Folstein,et al.  Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease , 1984, Neurology.

[19]  E. Tangalos,et al.  Mayo's older americans normative studies: WAIS-R norms for ages 56 to 97 , 1992 .

[20]  M. Sliwinski,et al.  Neuropsychological prediction of dementia and the absence of dementia in healthy elderly persons , 1994, Neurology.