The Quality of Spontaneous Movements of Preterm Infants: Associations with the Quality of Mother–Infant Interaction

The assessment of the quality of infants’ spontaneous movements is a diagnostic tool for the young nervous system. We examined whether it relates to the quality of the interactions between infants born preterm and their mothers. Thirty-nine healthy infants born preterm (Mage in weeks = 14.59, SD = 2.21; 38.46% female) and their mothers participated in the study. Infants’ quality of spontaneous movements was assessed using the General Movement Assessment according to Prechtl (Prechtl, Early Human Development 1990, 23, 151). A new measurement was employed to evaluate the following aspects of the mother–infant interaction: maternal sensitivity in the motor modality (i.e., when touching, picking up, holding, and putting down the infant), maternal sensitivity in the modalities of vocalization and eye contact, and infants’ positive engagement. Several aspects of infants’ quality of movements were associated with maternal sensitivity in the motor modality and infants’ positive engagement, but not with maternal sensitivity in the modalities of vocalization and eye contact. These findings suggest that the quality of infants’ spontaneous movements may explain some of the variability in the interactions between infants born preterm and their mothers. The results also highlight the importance of differentiating between the modalities of mothers’ behavior when assessing their sensitivity. Implications for research and practice with families of preterm infants are discussed.

[1]  J. Skranes,et al.  Follow-up at age 10 years in ELBW children - functional outcome, brain morphology and results from motor assessments in infancy. , 2014, Early human development.

[2]  A. Bos,et al.  Motor development in 3‐month‐old healthy term‐born infants is associated with cognitive and behavioural outcomes at early school age , 2014, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[3]  A. Schwartzman,et al.  Mutual touch during mother-infant face-to-face still-face interactions: influences of interaction period and infant birth status. , 2014, Infant behavior & development.

[4]  Della J. Derscheid,et al.  Emotional availability (EA): Theoretical background, empirical research using the EA Scales, and clinical applications , 2014 .

[5]  L. Lehtonen,et al.  Early relations between language development and the quality of mother-child interaction in very-low-birth-weight children. , 2014, Early human development.

[6]  Jeff Sigafoos,et al.  Highlighting the first 5 months of life: General movements in infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder or Rett Syndrome. , 2014, Research in autism spectrum disorders.

[7]  Betty Vohr,et al.  Adult Talk in the NICU With Preterm Infants and Developmental Outcomes , 2014, Pediatrics.

[8]  R. Feldman,et al.  Maternal-Preterm Skin-to-Skin Contact Enhances Child Physiologic Organization and Cognitive Control Across the First 10 Years of Life , 2014, Biological Psychiatry.

[9]  J. Skranes,et al.  Assessment of motor behaviour in high-risk-infants at 3 months predicts motor and cognitive outcomes in 10 years old children. , 2013, Early human development.

[10]  M. Jensen,et al.  Comparative Network Analysis of Preterm vs. Full-Term Infant-Mother Interactions , 2013, PloS one.

[11]  T. Kusaka,et al.  Extrauterine environment influences spontaneous low-frequency oscillations in the preterm brain , 2013, Brain and Development.

[12]  Giovanni Cioni,et al.  Early markers for cerebral palsy: insights from the assessment of general movements , 2012 .

[13]  A. Bos,et al.  Uncoordinated sucking patterns in preterm infants are associated with abnormal general movements. , 2012, The Journal of pediatrics.

[14]  P. Marschik,et al.  Cerebral palsy in children: Movements and postures during early infancy, dependent on preterm vs. full term birth , 2012, Early human development.

[15]  Rocco Micciolo,et al.  Poor repertoire General Movements predict some aspects of development outcome at 2 years in very preterm infants. , 2012, Early human development.

[16]  L. Lehtonen,et al.  The effects of preterm birth on mother–infant interaction and attachment during the infant's first two years , 2012, Acta obstetricia et gynecologica Scandinavica.

[17]  R. Boyd,et al.  Neonatal assessments for the preterm infant up to 4 months corrected age: a systematic review , 2012, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[18]  H. Roeyers,et al.  Gaze aversion during social interaction in preterm infants: a function of attention skills? , 2012, Infant behavior & development.

[19]  P. Marschik,et al.  Is it possible to predict the infant's neurodevelopmental outcome at 14 months of age by means of a single preterm assessment of General Movements? , 2012, Early human development.

[20]  R. Feldman,et al.  Neonatal brainstem dysfunction risks infant social engagement , 2011, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[21]  B. Vohr,et al.  Importance of Parent Talk on the Development of Preterm Infant Vocalizations , 2011, Pediatrics.

[22]  A. Bos,et al.  The motor repertoire of extremely low‐birthweight infants at term in relation to their neurological outcome , 2011, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[23]  A. Majnemer,et al.  Predictive validity of Prechtl’s Method on the Qualitative Assessment of General Movements: a systematic review of the evidence , 2011, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[24]  P. Marschik,et al.  Movements and postures of infants aged 3 to 5 months: to what extent is their optimality related to perinatal events and to the neurological outcome? , 2011, Early human development.

[25]  Ilanit Gordon,et al.  Gaze Behaviors of Preterm and Full-Term Infants in Nonsocial and Social Contexts of Increasing Dynamics: Visual Recognition, Attention Regulation, and Gaze Synchrony. , 2011, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[26]  Chien-Hui Lin,et al.  Development and initial validation of the Preschooler Gross Motor Quality Scale. , 2010, Research in developmental disabilities.

[27]  Joann L. Robinson,et al.  Maternal holding of preterm infants during the early weeks after birth and dyad interaction at six months. , 2010, Journal of obstetric, gynecologic, and neonatal nursing : JOGNN.

[28]  R. Borgatti,et al.  A comparison of dyadic interactions and coping with still-face in healthy pre-term and full-term infants. , 2010, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[29]  Carol Newnham,et al.  Early Sensitivity Training for Parents of Preterm Infants: Impact on the Developing Brain , 2010, Pediatric Research.

[30]  L. Doyle,et al.  Do early intervention programmes improve cognitive and motor outcomes for preterm infants after discharge? A systematic review , 2009, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[31]  G. Cioni,et al.  Early motor repertoire is related to level of self‐mobility in children with cerebral palsy at school age , 2009, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[32]  Q. Louw,et al.  The predictive validity of general movements--a systematic review. , 2009, European journal of paediatric neurology : EJPN : official journal of the European Paediatric Neurology Society.

[33]  A. Bouma,et al.  The quality of preterm infants' spontaneous movements: an early indicator of intelligence and behaviour at school age. , 2009, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[34]  Alan Fogel,et al.  A longitudinal investigation of maternal touching across the first 6 months of life: age and context effects. , 2009, Infant behavior & development.

[35]  R. Gur,et al.  Baby Schema in Infant Faces Induces Cuteness Perception and Motivation for Caretaking in Adults. , 2009, Ethology : formerly Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie.

[36]  L. Doyle,et al.  Parenting Behavior Is Associated With the Early Neurobehavioral Development of Very Preterm Children , 2009, Pediatrics.

[37]  L. Horwood,et al.  Development of emotional and behavioral regulation in children born extremely preterm and very preterm: biological and social influences. , 2008, Child development.

[38]  C. Einspieler,et al.  The quality of the early motor repertoire in preterm infants predicts minor neurologic dysfunction at school age. , 2008, The Journal of pediatrics.

[39]  R. Feldman,et al.  The development of maternal touch across the first year of life. , 2008, Early human development.

[40]  Barbara Mazer,et al.  A comparison of the general movements assessment with traditional approaches to newborn and infant assessment: concurrent validity. , 2008, Early human development.

[41]  L. Lehtonen,et al.  Mother-infant interaction is influenced by the amount of holding in preterm infants. , 2008, Early human development.

[42]  P. Marschik,et al.  Are abnormal fidgety movements an early marker for complex minor neurological dysfunction at puberty? , 2007, Early human development.

[43]  Ruth Feldman,et al.  Maternal postpartum behavior and the emergence of infant-mother and infant-father synchrony in preterm and full-term infants: the role of neonatal vagal tone. , 2007, Developmental psychobiology.

[44]  R. Feldman Maternal versus child risk and the development of parent–child and family relationships in five high-risk populations , 2007, Development and Psychopathology.

[45]  Christa Einspieler,et al.  Prechtl's assessment of general movements: a diagnostic tool for the functional assessment of the young nervous system. , 2005, Mental retardation and developmental disabilities research reviews.

[46]  François Ansermet,et al.  Prematurity, maternal stress and mother-child interactions. , 2004, Early human development.

[47]  M. Hadders‐Algra General movements: A window for early identification of children at high risk for developmental disorders. , 2004, The Journal of pediatrics.

[48]  Ruth Feldman,et al.  Testing a family intervention hypothesis: the contribution of mother-infant skin-to-skin contact (kangaroo care) to family interaction, proximity, and touch. , 2003, Journal of family psychology : JFP : journal of the Division of Family Psychology of the American Psychological Association.

[49]  Roger L. Brown,et al.  Infant and caregiving factors affecting weight-for-age and motor development of full-term and premature infants at 1 year post-term. , 2002, Research in nursing & health.

[50]  R. Feldman,et al.  Comparison of skin-to-skin (kangaroo) and traditional care: parenting outcomes and preterm infant development. , 2002, Pediatrics.

[51]  C. Einspieler,et al.  Can spontaneous movements be modulated by visual and acoustic stimulation in 3-month-old infants? , 2002, Early human development.

[52]  Giovanni Cioni,et al.  Cramped synchronized general movements in preterm infants as an early marker for cerebral palsy. , 2002, Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine.

[53]  R. Feldman,et al.  Skin-to-Skin contact (Kangaroo care) promotes self-regulation in premature infants: sleep-wake cyclicity, arousal modulation, and sustained exploration. , 2002, Developmental psychology.

[54]  J. Magill-Evans,et al.  Parent-Child Interactions, Parenting Stress, and Developmental Outcomes at 4 Years , 2001 .

[55]  G Cioni,et al.  The qualitative assessment of general movements in preterm, term and young infants--review of the methodology. , 1997, Early human development.

[56]  Giovanni Cioni,et al.  An early marker for neurological deficits after perinatal brain lesions , 1997, The Lancet.

[57]  L. Beckwith,et al.  Dyadic processes between mothers and preterm infants: Development at ages 2 to 5 years , 1996 .

[58]  C. Boesch,et al.  Structural and Neurobehavioral Delay in Postnatal Brain Development of Preterm Infants1 , 1996, Pediatric Research.

[59]  A. Fogel,et al.  Effect of postural position and reaching on gaze during mother-infant face-to-face interaction , 1992 .

[60]  H. Prechtl Qualitative changes of spontaneous movements in fetus and preterm infant are a marker of neurological dysfunction. , 1990, Early human development.

[61]  G. Cioni,et al.  Preterm and early postterm motor behaviour in low-risk premature infants. , 1990, Early human development.

[62]  M. Harrison A comparison of parental interactions with term and preterm infants. , 1990, Research in nursing & health.

[63]  K J Rothman,et al.  No Adjustments Are Needed for Multiple Comparisons , 1990, Epidemiology.

[64]  C. Corter,et al.  Maternal Responsiveness and Social Development in Preterm Infants. , 1989 .

[65]  E. Tronick Emotions and emotional communication in infants. , 1989, The American psychologist.

[66]  G. Anderson,et al.  Kangaroo care for premature infants. , 1986 .

[67]  L. Beckwith,et al.  Caregiver-Infant Interaction and Early Cognitive Development in Preterm Infants. , 1976 .

[68]  D. Winnicott The theory of the parent-infant relationship. , 1960, The International journal of psycho-analysis.

[69]  F. Ansermet,et al.  Prematurity, maternal posttraumatic stress and consequences on the mother-infant relationship. , 2011, Early human development.

[70]  H. Skouteris,et al.  Effectiveness of a modified Mother-Infant Transaction Program on outcomes for preterm infants from 3 to 24 months of age. , 2009, Infant behavior & development.

[71]  C. Einspieler,et al.  Quantitative aspects of the early motor repertoire in preterm infants: do they predict minor neurological dysfunction at school age? , 2009, Early human development.

[72]  A. Papageorgiou,et al.  Persistent maternal anxiety affects the interaction between mothers and their very low birthweight children at 24 months. , 2009, Early human development.

[73]  D. D. Raeymaecker Psychomotor development and psychopathology in childhood. , 2006 .

[74]  S. Weiss,et al.  Maternal Tactile Stimulation and the Neurodevelopment of Low Birth Weight Infants. , 2004 .

[75]  James F Troendle,et al.  Birthweight and gestational age effects on motor and social development. , 2002, Paediatric and perinatal epidemiology.

[76]  C. Trevarthen,et al.  Infant intersubjectivity: research, theory, and clinical applications. , 2001, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[77]  J. Hellmann,et al.  Impact of delayed development in premature infants on mother-infant interaction: a prospective investigation. , 1988, The Journal of pediatrics.

[78]  K. Grossmann,et al.  Maternal sensitivity and newborns' orientation responses as related to quality of attachment in northern Germany. , 1985, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.