Complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS) or continuous unilateral distal experimental pain stimulation in healthy subjects does not bias visual attention towards one hemifield

In natural life pain automatically draws attention towards the painful body part suggesting that it interacts with different attentional mechanisms such as visual attention. Complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS) patients who typically report on chronic distally located pain of one extremity may suffer from so-called neglect-like symptoms, which have also been linked to attentional mechanisms. The purpose of the study was to further evaluate how continuous pain conditions influence visual attention. Saccade latencies were recorded in two experiments using a common visual attention paradigm whereby orientating saccades to cued or uncued lateral visual targets had to be performed. In the first experiment saccade latencies of healthy subjects were measured under two conditions: one in which continuous experimental pain stimulation was applied to the index finger to imitate a continuous pain situation, and one without pain stimulation. In the second experiment saccade latencies of patients suffering from CRPS were compared to controls. The results showed that neither the continuous experimental pain stimulation during the experiment nor the chronic pain in CRPS led to an unilateral increase of saccade latencies or to a unilateral increase of the cue effect on latency. The results show that unilateral, continuously applied pain stimuli or chronic pain have no or only very limited influence on visual attention. Differently from patients with visual neglect, patients with CRPS did not show strong side asymmetries of saccade latencies or of cue effects on saccade latencies. Thus, neglect-like clinical symptoms of CRPS patients do not involve the allocation of visual attention.

[1]  William C. Ogden,et al.  Attended and unattended processing modes: The role of set for spatial location , 2014 .

[2]  G. Shulman,et al.  Moving attention through visual space. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  Lance M. Optican,et al.  Unix-based multiple-process system, for real-time data acquisition and control , 1982 .

[4]  M. Posner,et al.  Components of visual orienting , 1984 .

[5]  J. Findlay,et al.  The Relationship between Eye Movements and Spatial Attention , 1986, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[6]  G. Ratcliff,et al.  The disengagement of covert attention and the neglect syndrome , 1988, Psychobiology.

[7]  A. Milner,et al.  Line bisection errors in visual neglect: Misguided action or size distortion? , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  R. Abrams,et al.  Inhibition of return: effects of attentional cuing on eye movement latencies. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  H. Karnath,et al.  Subjective body orientation in neglect and the interactive contribution of neck muscle proprioception and vestibular stimulation. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[10]  B. Galer,et al.  Case reports and hypothesis: a neglect-like syndrome may be responsible for the motor disturbance in reflex sympathetic dystrophy (Complex Regional Pain Syndrome-1). , 1995, Journal of pain and symptom management.

[11]  J. Hoffman,et al.  The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[12]  R. Walker,et al.  Saccadic eye movement programming in unilateral neglect , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  A. K. Jones,et al.  Cerebral mechanisms operating in the presence and absence of inflammatory pain. , 1996, Annals of the rheumatic diseases.

[14]  B. Vogt,et al.  Pain Processing in Four Regions of Human Cingulate Cortex Localized with Co‐registered PET and MR Imaging , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.

[15]  M. L. Wood,et al.  Functional MRI of pain- and attention-related activations in the human cingulate cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  M. Bushnell,et al.  Pain affect encoded in human anterior cingulate but not somatosensory cortex. , 1997, Science.

[17]  W. Heide,et al.  Combined deficits of saccades and visuo-spatial orientation after cortical lesions , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  J. Dostrovsky,et al.  Pain-related neurons in the human cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[19]  R. Walker,et al.  A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[20]  W. Hernandez,et al.  Complex regional pain syndrome. , 1999, Journal of the American Podiatric Medical Association.

[21]  M. Urban,et al.  Central mechanisms in pain. , 1999, The Medical clinics of North America.

[22]  M. Fukumoto,et al.  Contralateral thalamic perfusion in patients with reflex sympathetic dystrophy syndrome , 1999, The Lancet.

[23]  M. Mesulam,et al.  Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal events. , 1999, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[24]  B. Galer,et al.  Neglect-like symptoms in complex regional pain syndrome: results of a self-administered survey. , 1999, Journal of pain and symptom management.

[25]  J. Maisog,et al.  Pain intensity processing within the human brain: a bilateral, distributed mechanism. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  M. Mesulam Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal events. , 1999, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[27]  A. Sereno,et al.  Inhibition of return in manual and saccadic response systems , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  R. Klein,et al.  Inhibition of return , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  H. Coslett,et al.  Pain and the body schema: evidence for peripheral effects on mental representations of movement. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[30]  R. Hari,et al.  Altered central sensorimotor processing in patients with complex regional pain syndrome , 2002, Pain.

[31]  R. Baron,et al.  Complex regional pain syndrome is a disease of the central nervous system , 2002, Clinical Autonomic Research.

[32]  Eugene McSorley,et al.  Distractor modulation of saccade trajectories: spatial separation and symmetry effects , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  G. Wasner,et al.  Complex regional pain syndrome – diagnostic, mechanisms, CNS involvement and therapy , 2003, Spinal Cord.

[34]  R. Baron,et al.  Complex regional pain syndrome: mystery explained? , 2003, The Lancet Neurology.

[35]  G. Moseley,et al.  Why do people with complex regional pain syndrome take longer to recognize their affected hand? , 2004, Neurology.

[36]  U. Sailer,et al.  Impaired self-perception of the hand in complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS) , 2004, Pain.

[37]  R. Abrams,et al.  The gap effect and inhibition of return: interactive effects on eye movement latencies , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  U. Sailer,et al.  Impaired temporal prediction and eye–hand coordination in patients with cerebellar lesions , 2005, Behavioural Brain Research.

[39]  Klaus Bartl,et al.  Eye movement driven head-mounted camera: it looks where the eyes look , 2005, 2005 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man and Cybernetics.

[40]  C. Maier,et al.  Severity and specificity of neglect-like symptoms in patients with complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS) compared to chronic limb pain of other origins , 2006, Pain.

[41]  R. Harden,et al.  Proposed new diagnostic criteria for complex regional pain syndrome. , 2007, Pain medicine.

[42]  C. McCabe,et al.  Body perception disturbance: A contribution to pain in complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS) , 2007, PAIN®.

[43]  Edgar Erdfelder,et al.  G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences , 2007, Behavior research methods.

[44]  G. Crombez,et al.  Pain draws visual attention to its location: experimental evidence for a threat-related bias. , 2007, The journal of pain : official journal of the American Pain Society.

[45]  G. Deuschl,et al.  The motor system shows adaptive changes in complex regional pain syndrome. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[46]  S. Miyauchi,et al.  Pathologic pain distorts visuospatial perception , 2007, Neurology.

[47]  M. Blankenburg,et al.  Left is where the L is right. Significantly delayed reaction time in limb laterality recognition in both CRPS and phantom limb pain patients , 2010, Neuroscience Letters.

[48]  C. Maihöfner,et al.  Complex regional pain syndromes: new pathophysiological concepts and therapies , 2010, European journal of neurology.

[49]  Stefan Van der Stigchel,et al.  The global effect: what determines where the eyes land? , 2011 .

[50]  Erik D. Reichle,et al.  Lexical and Post-Lexical Complexity Effects on Eye Movements in Reading. , 2011, Journal of eye movement research.

[51]  C. Maihöfner,et al.  Cognitive correlates of “neglect-like syndrome” in patients with complex regional pain syndrome , 2012, PAIN®.

[52]  O. Güntürkün,et al.  Impaired spatial body representation in complex regional pain syndrome type 1 (CRPS I) , 2012, PAIN®.

[53]  L. Noldus,et al.  Motor control in complex regional pain syndrome: A kinematic analysis , 2012, PAIN®.

[54]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. , 2012, Annual review of neuroscience.

[55]  B. Galer,et al.  Neglect-like signs and symptoms in CRPS , 2013, PAIN®.

[56]  P. Schwenkreis,et al.  Complex regional pain syndrome: more than a peripheral disease. , 2013, Pain management.