Numerical cognition in honeybees enables addition and subtraction

Honeybees learn to add or subtract one item from a set using color cues and can interpolate operations to a novel number. Many animals understand numbers at a basic level for use in essential tasks such as foraging, shoaling, and resource management. However, complex arithmetic operations, such as addition and subtraction, using symbols and/or labeling have only been demonstrated in a limited number of nonhuman vertebrates. We show that honeybees, with a miniature brain, can learn to use blue and yellow as symbolic representations for addition or subtraction. In a free-flying environment, individual bees used this information to solve unfamiliar problems involving adding or subtracting one element from a group of elements. This display of numerosity requires bees to acquire long-term rules and use short-term working memory. Given that honeybees and humans are separated by over 400 million years of evolution, our findings suggest that advanced numerical cognition may be more accessible to nonhuman animals than previously suspected.

[1]  M. Giurfa,et al.  The forest or the trees: preference for global over local image processing is reversed by prior experience in honeybees , 2015, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[2]  J. Call,et al.  Estimating and operating on discrete quantities in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). , 2000, Journal of comparative psychology.

[3]  M. Hauser,et al.  Can rhesus monkeys spontaneously subtract? , 2001, Cognition.

[4]  K. Wynn,et al.  Large-Number Addition and Subtraction by 9-Month-Old Infants , 2004, Psychological science.

[5]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  Evidence for counting in insects , 2008, Animal Cognition.

[6]  Lars Chittka,et al.  Can honey bees count landmarks? , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[7]  Michael J Beran,et al.  Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) respond to nonvisible sets after one-by-one addition and removal of items. , 2004, Journal of comparative psychology.

[8]  M. Giurfa,et al.  Conceptual learning by miniature brains , 2013, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[9]  T. Verguts,et al.  Numerical Cognition: Learning Binds Biology to Culture , 2017, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[11]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  The concepts of ‘sameness’ and ‘difference’ in an insect , 2001, Nature.

[12]  J. Tautz,et al.  Number-Based Visual Generalisation in the Honeybee , 2009, PloS one.

[13]  Andreas Nieder Honey bees zero in on the empty set , 2018, Science.

[14]  A. Nieder Number Faculty Is Rooted in Our Biological Heritage , 2017, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  Adrian G. Dyer,et al.  Free-flying honeybees extrapolate relational size rules to sort successively visited artificial flowers in a realistic foraging situation , 2017, Animal Cognition.

[16]  M. Beran,et al.  Chimpanzees Remember the Results of One-by-One Addition of Food Items to Sets Over Extended Time Periods , 2004, Psychological science.

[17]  J. Niven,et al.  Are Bigger Brains Better? , 2009, Current Biology.

[18]  Lars Chittka,et al.  Honeybee (Apis mellifera) vision can discriminate between and recognise images of human faces , 2005, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[19]  M. Giurfa,et al.  Aversive Reinforcement Improves Visual Discrimination Learning in Free-Flying Honeybees , 2010, PloS one.

[20]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  Maze navigation by honeybees: learning path regularity. , 2000, Learning & memory.

[21]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  Maze Learning by Honeybees , 1996, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[22]  R. Núñez,et al.  Is There Really an Evolved Capacity for Number? , 2017, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[23]  Karen Wynn,et al.  Addition and subtraction by human infants , 1992, Nature.

[24]  M. Srinivasan Honey bees as a model for vision, perception, and cognition. , 2010, Annual review of entomology.

[25]  A. Nieder Counting on neurons: the neurobiology of numerical competence , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[26]  Adrian G. Dyer,et al.  Conceptualization of relative size by honeybees , 2014, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[27]  M. Giurfa,et al.  Conceptualization of above and below relationships by an insect , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[28]  X. Nelson,et al.  The role of numerical competence in a specialized predatory strategy of an araneophagic spider , 2012, Animal Cognition.

[29]  Michael D. Dodd,et al.  Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[30]  Jair E. Garcia,et al.  Numerical ordering of zero in honey bees , 2018, Science.

[31]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Exact and Approximate Arithmetic in an Amazonian Indigene Group , 2004, Science.

[32]  G. Berntson,et al.  Numerical competence in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). , 1989, Journal of comparative psychology.

[33]  P. Skorupski,et al.  Counting insects , 2018, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[34]  I. Pepperberg Further evidence for addition and numerical competence by a Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus) , 2012, Animal Cognition.

[35]  Adrian G. Dyer,et al.  Perception of contextual size illusions by honeybees in restricted and unrestricted viewing conditions , 2017, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[36]  Gerlinde Höbel,et al.  Nephila clavipes spiders (Araneae: Nephilidae) keep track of captured prey counts: testing for a sense of numerosity in an orb-weaver , 2014, Animal Cognition.

[37]  J. J. Franks,et al.  Effort toward comprehension: Elaboration or “aha”? , 1979 .

[38]  I. Pepperberg Grey Parrot (Psittacus erithacus) numerical abilities: addition and further experiments on a zero-like concept. , 2006, Journal of comparative psychology.

[39]  K. Fujita,et al.  Arithmetic-Like Reasoning in Wild Vervet Monkeys: A Demonstration of Cost-Benefit Calculation in Foraging , 2011 .

[40]  C R Gallistel,et al.  Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale , 2001, Psychological science.