The Progress Bias in Goal Pursuit: When One Step Forward Seems Larger than One Step Back

Consumers often pursue goals (e.g., losing weight) where the chance of attaining the goal increases with some behaviors (e.g., exercise) but decreases with others (e.g., eating). Although goal monitoring is known to be a critical step in self-control for successful goal pursuit, little research investigates whether consumers accurately monitor goal progress. Seven experiments demonstrate that consumers tend to show a progress bias in goal monitoring, perceiving that goal-consistent behaviors (e.g., saving $45) help progress more than goal-inconsistent behaviors of the equivalent size (e.g., spending $45) hurt it. Expectations of goal attainment moderate the progress bias; reducing the expectation that the goal will be reached reduces the tendency to perceive goal-consistent behaviors to have a larger impact on goal progress than equivalent goal-inconsistent behaviors. A study on exercise and eating shows that although the progress bias can increase initial goal persistence, it can also lead to premature goal release due to poor calibration of overall progress.

[1]  Rik Pieters,et al.  Dynamics of multiple-goal pursuit. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[2]  J. Polivy,et al.  The effects of self-attention and public attention on eating in restrained and unrestrained subjects. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[3]  Andrea Bonezzi,et al.  Stuck in the Middle , 2011, Psychological science.

[4]  David R. Bassett,et al.  Energy cost of stair climbing and descending on the college alumnus questionnaire. , 1997 .

[5]  E. A. Locke,et al.  Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation. A 35-year odyssey. , 2002, The American psychologist.

[6]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect Theory : An Analysis of Decision under Risk Author ( s ) : , 2007 .

[7]  P. Krishnamurthy,et al.  Resisting That Triple-Chocolate Cake: Mental Budgets and Self-Control , 2010 .

[8]  N. Weinstein Unrealistic optimism about future life events , 1980 .

[9]  Kurt A. Carlson,et al.  Unrealistically Optimistic Consumers: A Selective Hypothesis Testing Account for Optimism in Predictions of Future Behavior , 2009 .

[10]  R. Pieters,et al.  Goal-directed consumer behavior: Motivation, volition, and affect. , 2008 .

[11]  Joseph C. Nunes,et al.  The Endowed Progress Effect: How Artificial Advancement Increases Effort , 2006 .

[12]  J. Polivy,et al.  If at first you don't succeed. False hopes of self-change. , 2002, The American psychologist.

[13]  A. Bandura Human agency in social cognitive theory. , 1989, The American psychologist.

[14]  L. Green,et al.  Commitment using punishment. , 1996, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[15]  Chris Janiszewski,et al.  Context-Dependent Effects of Goal Primes , 2008 .

[16]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect theory: analysis of decision under risk , 1979 .

[17]  C. Carver,et al.  On the Self-Regulation of Behavior , 1998 .

[18]  Ran Kivetz,et al.  The Goal-Gradient Hypothesis Resurrected: Purchase Acceleration, Illusionary Goal Progress, and Customer Retention , 2006 .

[19]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty , 1982 .

[20]  D. Wentura,et al.  Immunizing the Self: Self-Concept Stabilization Through Reality-Adaptive Self-Definitions , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[21]  Arie W. Kruglanski,et al.  The dynamics of consumer behavior: A goal systemic perspective , 2012 .

[22]  David Dunning,et al.  The Mixed Blessings of Self-Knowledge in Behavioral Prediction: Enhanced Discrimination but Exacerbated Bias , 2006, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[23]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Self-Regulation Failure: An Overview , 1996 .

[24]  Leslie F. Clark,et al.  RATING SCALES NUMERIC VALUES MAY CHANGE THE MEANING OF SCALE LABELS , 1991 .

[25]  T. K. Srull,et al.  Memory and Cognition in Its Social Context , 1989 .

[26]  Ying Zhang,et al.  So near and yet so far: the mental representation of goal progress. , 2012, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[27]  R. Buehler,et al.  Exploring the "planning fallacy": Why people underestimate their task completion times. , 1994 .

[28]  Ying Zhang,et al.  The dilution model: how additional goals undermine the perceived instrumentality of a shared path. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[29]  J. Klayman,et al.  Confirmation, Disconfirmation, and Informa-tion in Hypothesis Testing , 1987 .

[30]  Ying Zhang,et al.  How Endowed versus Earned Progress Affects Consumer Goal Commitment and Motivation , 2010 .

[31]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.

[32]  Ravi Dhar,et al.  Goals as Excuses or Guides: The Liberating Effect of Perceived Goal Progress on Choice , 2005 .

[33]  A. Tremblay,et al.  Effects of exercise on appetite control: implications for energy balance. , 1997, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[34]  C. Carver,et al.  Control theory: a useful conceptual framework for personality-social, clinical, and health psychology. , 1982, Psychological bulletin.

[35]  Ştefan Boncu,et al.  E. Tory Higgins si Arie Kruglanski (coord.), Social psychology. Handbook of basic principles, Guilford, New York, 1997 , 1998 .

[36]  Kristopher J Preacher,et al.  Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models , 2008, Behavior research methods.

[37]  P. Gollwitzer,et al.  Implementation intentions and effective goal pursuit , 1997 .

[38]  Bärbel Knäuper,et al.  Compensatory health beliefs: scale development and psychometric properties , 2004 .

[39]  A. Kruglanski Motivated social cognition: Principles of the interface. , 1996 .

[40]  Loran F. Nordgren,et al.  The Restraint Bias: How the Illusion of Self-Restraint Promotes Impulsive Behavior , 2009, Psychological science.

[41]  Preferences Scientific Kahneman, Daniel, and Amos Tversky. , 1982 .

[42]  K. Vohs,et al.  Self-regulation: Goals, consumption, and choices , 2008 .

[43]  B. Knäuper,et al.  The eternal quest for optimal balance between maximizing pleasure and minimizing harm: the compensatory health beliefs model. , 2006, British journal of health psychology.

[44]  B. Fischhoff,et al.  Behavioral Decision Theory , 1977 .

[45]  David Dunning,et al.  Egocentric Definitions of Traits and Abilities in Social Judgment , 1992 .

[46]  B. Wansink,et al.  Just thinking about exercise makes me serve more food. Physical activity and calorie compensation , 2011, Appetite.

[47]  A. Tesser,et al.  The "What the Hell" Effect: Some Effects of Goal Proximity and Goal Framing on Performance , 1996 .

[48]  Chris Janiszewski,et al.  A Goal-Based Model of Product Evaluation and Choice , 2012 .

[49]  Jeff T. Larsen,et al.  Comparing Gains and Losses , 2010, Psychological science.

[50]  R. Nickerson Confirmation Bias: A Ubiquitous Phenomenon in Many Guises , 1998 .

[51]  G. Colletti,et al.  Maintaining behavior change: in investigation of three maintenance strategies and the relationship of self-attribution to the long-term reduction of cigarette smoking. , 1979, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[52]  Edward B. Royzman,et al.  Negativity Bias, Negativity Dominance, and Contagion , 2001 .

[53]  Minjung Koo,et al.  The Small-Area Hypothesis: Effects of Progress Monitoring on Goal Adherence , 2012 .

[54]  A. Fishbach,et al.  Counteracting obstacles with optimistic predictions. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[55]  K. Vohs,et al.  Case Western Reserve University , 1990 .