Affective blindsight: intact fear conditioning to a visual cue in a cortically blind patient.

Blindsight refers to remarkable residual visual abilities of patients with damage to the primary visual cortex (V1). Recent studies revealed that such residual abilities do not apply only to relatively simple object discriminations, but that these patients can also differentially categorize and respond to emotionally salient stimuli. The current study reports on a case of intact fear conditioning to a visual cue in a male patient with complete bilateral cortical blindness. The patient was admitted to the stroke unit of the neurological department because of complete loss of vision. Both CT and structural MRI scans confirmed lesions in both territories of the posterior cerebral artery. No visual evoked potentials could be detected confirming complete cortical blindness. During fear conditioning, a visual cue predicted the occurrence of an aversive electric shock. Acoustic startle probes were presented during and between the conditioned stimuli. Relative to the control condition, startle reflexes were substantially potentiated when elicited in the presence of the conditioned stimuli. No such potentiation was observed prior to conditioning. These data suggest that fear learning to visual cues does not require a cortical representation of the conditioned stimulus in the primary sensory cortex and that subcortical pathways are sufficient to activate the fear module in humans.

[1]  R. D. Kavanaugh,et al.  Emotion : interdisciplinary perspectives , 1996 .

[2]  E. G. Jones,et al.  A projection from the medial pulvinar to the amygdala in primates , 1976, Brain Research.

[3]  S. Mineka,et al.  Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. , 2001, Psychological review.

[4]  Michael Davis,et al.  Visual cortex ablations do not prevent extinction of fear-potentiated startle using a visual conditioned stimulus. , 1993, Behavioral and neural biology.

[5]  W. K. Berg,et al.  Startle Modification: Startle Elicitation: Stimulus Parameters, Recording Techniques, and Quantification , 1999 .

[6]  L. Weiskrantz,et al.  Differential extrageniculostriate and amygdala responses to presentation of emotional faces in a cortically blind field. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  A. Cowey,et al.  Wavelength discrimination in blindsight. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[8]  Michael Davis,et al.  Are different parts of the extended amygdala involved in fear versus anxiety? , 1998, Biological Psychiatry.

[9]  Michael Davis,et al.  The role of the amygdala in conditioned fear. , 1992 .

[10]  B. Sabel,et al.  Visual field enlargement after computer training in brain-damaged patients with homonymous deficits: an open pilot trial. , 1995, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.

[11]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Information cascade from primary auditory cortex to the amygdala: corticocortical and corticoamygdaloid projections of temporal cortex in the rat. , 1993, Cerebral cortex.

[12]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Topographic organization of neurons in the acoustic thalamus that project to the amygdala , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[13]  Gilles Pourtois,et al.  Fear recognition in the voice is modulated by unconsciously recognized facial expressions but not by unconsciously recognized affective pictures , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[14]  Michael Davis The neurophysiological basis of acoustic startle modulation: Research on fear motivation and sensory gating. , 1997 .

[15]  P. Lang,et al.  Accuracy of psychophysical judgments and physiological response amplitude. , 1973, Journal of experimental psychology.

[16]  L. Nadel,et al.  Cognitive neuroscience of emotion , 2000 .

[17]  David G. Amaral,et al.  The role of the amygdala in primate social cognition. , 2000 .

[18]  K H Ruddock,et al.  Human visual responses in the absence of the geniculo-calcarine projection. , 1980, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[19]  R. Dolan,et al.  Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala , 1998, Nature.

[20]  F. Graham,et al.  Constraints on measuring heart rate and period sequentially through real and cardiac time. , 1978, Psychophysiology.

[21]  L Weiskrantz,et al.  Non-conscious recognition of affect in the absence of striate cortex. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[22]  E. Edgington,et al.  Randomization Tests (3rd ed.) , 1998 .

[23]  M. Bradley,et al.  Emotional learning, hedonic change, and the startle probe. , 1993, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[24]  H. Pape,et al.  Direct synaptic connections of axons from superior colliculus with identified thalamo‐amygdaloid projection neurons in the rat: Possible substrates of a subcortical visual pathway to the amygdala , 1999, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[25]  Joseph E LeDoux The emotional brain , 1996 .

[26]  R. Dolan,et al.  A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating "unseen" fear. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[27]  O V Lipp,et al.  Human blink startle during aversive and nonaversive Pavlovian conditioning. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[28]  Jens Frahm,et al.  COMMENTS AND CONTROVERSIES Functional MRI of the Human Amygdala , 2001 .

[29]  Joseph E. LeDoux,et al.  Cognitive–emotional interactions: Listen to the brain. , 2000 .

[30]  A. Hamm,et al.  Affective learning: awareness and aversion. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[31]  Bede Rundle,et al.  Consciousness Lost and Found: A Neuropsychological Exploration , 2004 .