Chemosensory conditioning in molluscs: II. A critical review

We critically review chemosensory conditioning studies with molluscs and find that, in many studies, the influence of nonassociative processes complicates, obscures, and renders ambiguous the unique contribution of associative learning. These nonassociative processes include sensory adaptation, habituation, sensitization, and changes in feeding motivation. They arise from both the food extracts that have often been used as conditioned stimuli and the aversive stimuli that have been used as unconditioned stimuli.

[1]  K. R. Weiss,et al.  The effects of food arousal on the latency of biting inAplysia , 1978, Journal of comparative physiology.

[2]  L. Matzel,et al.  Neurophysiological Substrates of Context Conditioning in Hermissenda Suggest a Temporally Invariant Form of Activity-Dependent Neuronal Facilitation , 1999, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[3]  R. M. Lee,et al.  Behavioral states and feeding in the gastropod Pleurobranchaea. , 1976, Behavioral biology.

[4]  R M Colwill,et al.  Context-US learning in Aplysia californica , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[5]  R. Colwill,et al.  Pavlovian appetitive discriminative conditioning inAplysia californica , 1997 .

[6]  Neophobia and water intake after repeated pairings of novel flavors with toxicosis , 1980, Physiology & Behavior.

[7]  J. Pearce,et al.  Inhibitory interactions between appetitive and aversive stimuli. , 1977 .

[8]  E. Kandel Cellular basis of behavior: An introduction to behavioral neurobiology. , 1976 .

[9]  A. D. McClellan,et al.  Learning: a model system for physiological studies. , 1978, Science.

[10]  J. Farley,et al.  Potentiation of phototactic suppression in Hermissenda by a chemosensory stimulus during compound conditioning. , 1997, Behavioral neuroscience.

[11]  Eric R. Kandel,et al.  Long-Term Sensitization of a Defensive Withdrawal Reflex in Aplysia , 1973, Science.

[12]  W. Davis,et al.  Learning: Classical and Avoidance Conditioning in the Mollusk Pleurobranchaea , 1973, Science.

[13]  A. Gelperin,et al.  An analysis of associative learning in a terrestrial mollusc , 1981, Journal of comparative physiology.

[14]  J. Rzóska Bait shyness, a study in rat behaviour , 1953 .

[15]  J. Farley,et al.  Chemosensory conditioning in molluscs: I. Failure of contextual conditioning inHermissenda , 2004, Learning & behavior.

[16]  A. Gelperin,et al.  Odors can induce feeding motor responses in the terrestrial mollusc Limax maximus. , 1992, Behavioral neuroscience.

[17]  V. Lolordo,et al.  Reexamination of contextual conditioning with massed versus distributed unconditioned stimuli , 1991 .

[18]  J. Konorski Integrative activity of the brain , 1967 .

[19]  W. Davis,et al.  The behavioral hierarchy of the molluskPleurobranchaea , 1974, Journal of comparative physiology.

[20]  M. Kovac,et al.  Command neurons in Pleurobranchaea receive synaptic feedback from the motor network they excite. , 1978, Science.

[21]  E. Kandel,et al.  Classical conditioning in Aplysia californica. , 1979, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[22]  C. Mccrohan,et al.  FOOD-RELATED CONDITIONING AND NEURONAL CORRELATES IN THE FRESHWATER SNAIL LYMNAEA STAGNALIS , 1996 .

[23]  G. Kemenes,et al.  Training in a novel environment improves the appetitive learning performance of the snail, Lymnaea stagnalis. , 1994, Behavioral and neural biology.

[24]  R. Gillette,et al.  Cost-benefit analysis potential in feeding behavior of a predatory snail by integration of hunger, taste, and pain. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[25]  P. Benjamin,et al.  Appetitive learning in snails shows characteristics of conditioning in vertebrates , 1989, Brain Research.

[26]  G. Shepherd,et al.  Mechanisms of olfactory discrimination: converging evidence for common principles across phyla. , 1997, Annual review of neuroscience.

[27]  D. Alkon Associative Training of Hermissenda , 1974, The Journal of general physiology.

[28]  R. Gilbert Animal Discrimination Learning , 1969 .

[29]  M. Kovac,et al.  Food avoidance learning is accompanied by synaptic attenuation in identified interneurons controlling feeding behavior in Pleurobranchaea. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  E Feldman,et al.  Learned changes of feeding behavior in Aplysia in response to edible and inedible foods , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[31]  Learning: Neural analysis in the isolated brain of a previously trained mollusc,Pleurobranchaea californica , 1985, Brain Research.

[32]  J. Farley,et al.  Potentiation of phototactic suppression in Hermissenda by compound conditioning results in potentiated excitability changes in type B and A photoreceptors. , 1997, Behavioral neuroscience.

[33]  R. Ostfeld,et al.  The Behavioral Hierarchy of the Garden Snail Helix aspersa , 1982 .

[34]  H. M. Jenkins,et al.  Effect of discrimination training on auditory generalization. , 1960, Journal of experimental psychology.

[35]  H. M. Hanson Effects of discrimination training on stimulus generalization. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.

[36]  A. Gelperin,et al.  One-trial associative learning modifies food odor preferences of a terrestrial mollusc. , 1981, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[37]  A. Gelperin Rapid food-aversion learning by a terrestrial mollusk. , 1975, Science.

[38]  L. Matzel,et al.  Higher-order associative processing in Hermissenda suggests multiple sites of neuronal modulation. , 1996, Learning & memory.

[39]  J. Farley,et al.  Training and testing determinants of short-term associative suppression of phototaxic behavior in Hermissenda. , 1987, Behavioral and neural biology.

[40]  N. Mackintosh,et al.  Mechanisms of animal discrimination learning , 1971 .

[41]  M. Anderson,et al.  Singleness of action in the interactions of feeding with other behaviors in Hermissenda crassicornis. , 1988, Behavioral and neural biology.

[42]  Christie L. Sahley,et al.  Analysis of associative learning in the terrestrial mollusc Limax maximus. II. Appetitive learning , 1990, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[43]  W. Hankins,et al.  Flavor-illness aversions: potentiation of odor by taste in rats. , 1979, Behavioral and neural biology.

[44]  W. Davis,et al.  Selective and differential avoidance learning in the feeding and withdrawal behavior ofPleuobranchaea californica , 1980, Journal of comparative physiology.

[45]  D. Mitchell,et al.  Attenuated and enhanced neophobia in the taste-aversion “delay of reinforcement” effect , 1977 .

[46]  Modulation of defensive reflexes in Aplysia californica by appetitive stimulation. , 1980, Behavioral and neural biology.

[47]  Chemosensory conditioning of Hermissenda crassicornis. , 1990 .

[48]  L. Matzel,et al.  Chemosensory-based contextual conditioning inHermissenda crassicornis , 1996 .

[49]  R. Rescorla,et al.  Two-process learning theory: Relationships between Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning. , 1967, Psychological review.

[50]  G. Audesirk,et al.  Rapid, nonaversive conditioning in a freshwater gastropod. I. Effects of age and motivation. , 1982, Behavioral and neural biology.

[51]  D. Alkon,et al.  Interaction of chemosensory, visual, and statocyst pathways in Hermissenda crassicornis , 1978, The Journal of general physiology.

[52]  M. Wells,et al.  Conditioning and sensitization in snails. , 1971, Animal behaviour.

[53]  N. Braveman,et al.  Independence of neophobia and taste aversion learning , 1978 .

[54]  P. Balaban Postsynaptic mechanism of withdrawal reflex sensitization in the snail. , 1983, Journal of neurobiology.

[55]  R. Yerkes,et al.  The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit‐formation , 1908 .

[56]  C. Mccrohan,et al.  Food arousal in the pond snail, Lymnaea stagnalis. , 1987, Behavioral and neural biology.

[57]  P. Balsam,et al.  The effects of varying the interreinforcement interval on appetitive contextual conditioning , 1991 .

[58]  H. M. Jenkins,et al.  Generalization gradients of inhibition following auditory discrimination learning. , 1962, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[59]  M C Nelson,et al.  Classical conditioning in the blowfly (Phormia regina): associative and excitatory factors. , 1971, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[60]  M. Bitterman,et al.  Classical conditioning of proboscis extension in honeybees (Apis mellifera). , 1983, Journal of comparative psychology.