A Comparison of Visual and Nonvisual Sensory Inputs to Walked Distance in a Blind-Walking Task
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] A. S. Gilinsky. Perceived size and distance in visual space. , 1951, Psychological review.
[2] Heinz Werner,et al. Changes in Phychological Distance under Conditions of Danger1 , 1955 .
[3] D. H. Warren,et al. Sensory conflict in judgments of spatial direction , 1969 .
[4] D. H. Warren,et al. Intermodality relations in localization in blind and sighted people , 1970 .
[5] L Weiskrantz,et al. Varieties of Residual Experience* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[6] J. Thomson. Is continuous visual monitoring necessary in visually guided locomotion? , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[7] D. Elliott. Continuous visual information may be important after all: a failure to replicate Thomson (1983) , 1986 .
[8] D. Elliott. Continuous visual information may be important after all: a failure to replicate Thomson (1983). , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[9] M A Goodale,et al. The effects of time and distance on accuracy of target-directed locomotion: does an accurate short-term memory for spatial location exist? , 1988, Journal of motor behavior.
[10] J. Antes,et al. The Effect of a New City Traffic Route on the Cognitive Maps of Its Residents , 1988 .
[11] A. Berthoz,et al. Contribution of the otoliths to the calculation of linear displacement. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.
[12] R. Klatzky,et al. Acquisition of route and survey knowledge in the absence of vision. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.
[13] J. Rieser,et al. Visual Perception and the Guidance of Locomotion without Vision to Previously Seen Targets , 1990, Perception.
[14] W. Meck,et al. The organizational effects of gonadal steroids on sexually dimorphic spatial ability , 1991, Psychoneuroendocrinology.
[15] Thomas G. Bever. The logical and extrinsic sources of modularity. , 1992 .
[16] T. Mergner,et al. Interaction of vestibular and proprioceptive inputs for human self-motion perception , 1992, Neuroscience Letters.
[17] J. Loomis,et al. Visual space perception and visually directed action. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[18] R. Klatzky,et al. Nonvisual navigation by blind and sighted: assessment of path integration ability. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[19] M. Goodale,et al. The visual brain in action , 1995 .
[20] R. Klatzky,et al. Performance of Blind and Sighted Persons on Spatial Tasks , 1995 .
[21] S S Fukusima,et al. Visual perception of egocentric distance as assessed by triangulation. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[22] A Berthoz,et al. Spatial memory and path integration studied by self-driven passive linear displacement. I. Basic properties. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.
[23] J. M. Dabbs,et al. Spatial Ability, Navigation Strategy, and Geographic Knowledge Among Men and Women , 1998 .
[24] S. Moffat,et al. Navigation in a “Virtual” Maze: Sex Differences and Correlation With Psychometric Measures of Spatial Ability in Humans , 1998 .
[25] F Danion,et al. Veering in human locomotion: the role of the effectors , 1999, Neuroscience Letters.
[26] M. Fisher,et al. Evolved mechanisms underlying wayfinding. further studies on the hunter-gatherer theory of spatial sex differences. , 2000, Evolution and human behavior : official journal of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society.
[27] C. Ellard,et al. The contribution of nonvisual information to simple place navigation and distance estimation: an examination of path integration. , 2000, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.
[28] M Behrmann,et al. Active control of locomotion facilitates nonvisual navigation. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[29] Horst Mittelstaedt,et al. Idiothetic navigation in humans: estimation of path length , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.
[30] M. Ernst,et al. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.
[31] D. Wolpert,et al. When Feeling Is More Important Than Seeing in Sensorimotor Adaptation , 2002, Current Biology.
[32] Alain Berthoz,et al. Interaction of visual and idiothetic information in a path completion task , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[33] J. Philbeck,et al. Dissociation between location and shape in visual space. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[34] E. Holst,et al. Das Reafferenzprinzip , 2004, Naturwissenschaften.
[35] A. Berthoz,et al. Goal-directed linear locomotion in normal and labyrinthine-defective subjects , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.