When holding your horses meets the deer in the headlights: time-frequency characteristics of global and selective stopping under conditions of proactive and reactive control

The ability to inhibit unwanted thoughts or actions is crucial for successful functioning in daily life; however, this ability is often impaired in a number of psychiatric disorders. Despite the relevance of inhibition in everyday situations, current models of inhibition are rather simplistic and provide little generalizability especially in the face of clinical disorders. Thus, given the importance of inhibition for proper cognitive functioning, the need for a paradigm, which incorporates factors that will subsequently improve the current model for understanding inhibition, is of high demand. A popular paradigm used to assess motor inhibition, the stop-signal paradigm, can be modified to further advance the current conceptual model of inhibitory control and thus provide a basis for better understanding different facets of inhibition. Namely, in this study, we have developed a novel version of the stop-signal task to assess how preparation (that is, whether reactive or proactive) and selectivity of the stopping behavior effect well-known time-frequency characteristics associated with successful inhibition and concomitant behavioral measures. With this innovative paradigm, we demonstrate that the selective nature of the stopping task modulates theta and motoric beta activity and we further provide the first account of delta activity as an electrophysiological feature sensitive to both manipulations of selectivity and preparatory control.

[1]  Christina F. Lavallee,et al.  Electroencephalography of response inhibition tasks: functional networks and cognitive contributions. , 2013, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[2]  R. Kahn,et al.  Reduced Proactive Inhibition in Schizophrenia Is Related to Corticostriatal Dysfunction and Poor Working Memory , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.

[3]  Michael A. DiSano,et al.  Intracranial EEG Reveals a Time- and Frequency-Specific Role for the Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus and Primary Motor Cortex in Stopping Initiated Responses , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Behavioral/systems/cognitive Effective Connectivity Reveals Important Roles for Both the Hyperdirect (fronto-subthalamic) and the Indirect (fronto-striatal-pallidal) Fronto-basal Ganglia Pathways during Response Inhibition , 2022 .

[5]  J. Artieda,et al.  Oscillatory changes related to the forced termination of a movement , 2008, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[6]  A. Aron,et al.  Stop the Presses , 2008, Psychological science.

[7]  Christo Pantev,et al.  Conflict and inhibition differentially affect the N200/P300 complex in a combined go/nogo and stop-signal task , 2010, NeuroImage.

[8]  Jan R. Wessel,et al.  EEG signatures associated with stopping are sensitive to preparation. , 2013, Psychophysiology.

[9]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  Single-Trial Normalization for Event-Related Spectral Decomposition Reduces Sensitivity to Noisy Trials , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[10]  G. V. Boxtel,et al.  Development of response activation and inhibition in a selective stop-signal task , 2014, Biological Psychology.

[11]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[12]  R. Nigbur,et al.  Theta power as a marker for cognitive interference , 2011, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[13]  Russell A. Poldrack,et al.  The Cognitive Neuroscience of Response Inhibition: Relevance for Genetic Research in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.

[14]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Preparation to Inhibit a Response Complements Response Inhibition during Performance of a Stop-Signal Task , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  Jan R Wessel,et al.  Unexpected Events Induce Motor Slowing via a Brain Mechanism for Action-Stopping with Global Suppressive Effects , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  G Pfurtscheller,et al.  Effects of handedness on movement-related changes of central beta rhythms. , 1997, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[17]  W. Byblow,et al.  Intracortical inhibition during volitional inhibition of prepared action. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  M. Coltheart Hemispheric asymmetry , 1978, Nature.

[19]  René Westerhausen,et al.  Corticospinal tract asymmetries at the level of the internal capsule: Is there an association with handedness? , 2007, NeuroImage.

[20]  Caitlin L. Oldenkamp,et al.  A Proactive Mechanism for Selective Suppression of Response Tendencies , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  B. Sahakian,et al.  The neuropsychiatry of impulsivity , 2007, Current opinion in psychiatry.

[22]  Gordon D Logan,et al.  Horse-race model simulations of the stop-signal procedure. , 2003, Acta psychologica.

[23]  Anna Grabowska,et al.  Response inhibition of children with ADHD in the stop-signal task: an event-related potential study. , 2012, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[24]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  Enhanced detection of artifacts in EEG data using higher-order statistics and independent component analysis , 2007, NeuroImage.

[25]  Yoshiharu Yamamoto,et al.  Single-trial EEG Power and Phase Dynamics Associated with Voluntary Response Inhibition , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  Jobi S. George,et al.  Transcranial magnetic stimulation reveals dissociable mechanisms for global versus selective corticomotor suppression underlying the stopping of action. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  Y. Smith,et al.  Microcircuitry of the direct and indirect pathways of the basal ganglia. , 1998, Neuroscience.

[28]  U. Ziemann,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetry of transcallosal inhibition in man. , 1995, Experimental brain research.

[29]  Stephen Darling,et al.  Intraindividual reaction time variability affects P300 amplitude rather than latency , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[30]  L. J. Anthony,et al.  The Cambridge Dictionary of Statistics (2nd ed.) , 2003 .

[31]  Christina F. Lavallee,et al.  Stimulus-Response Mappings Shape Inhibition Processes: A Combined EEG-fMRI Study of Contextual Stopping , 2014, PloS one.

[32]  Trevor W Robbins,et al.  Motor inhibition and cognitive flexibility in obsessive-compulsive disorder and trichotillomania. , 2006, The American journal of psychiatry.

[33]  U. Ziemann,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetry of transcallosalinhibition in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  G. Buzsáki,et al.  Theta Oscillations Provide Temporal Windows for Local Circuit Computation in the Entorhinal-Hippocampal Loop , 2009, Neuron.

[35]  E. Basar,et al.  P300-response: possible psychophysiological correlates in delta and theta frequency channels. A review. , 1992, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[36]  T. Robbins,et al.  A componential analysis of task-switching deficits associated with lesions of left and right frontal cortex. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[37]  E. Bernat,et al.  Theta and delta band activity explain N2 and P3 ERP component activity in a go/no-go task , 2014, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[38]  J. Polich Updating P 300 : An Integrative Theory of P 3 a and P 3 b , 2009 .

[39]  J. Polich Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[40]  T. Braver The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[41]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibition and impulsivity: Behavioral and neural basis of response control , 2013, Progress in Neurobiology.

[42]  A. Engel,et al.  Beta-band oscillations—signalling the status quo? , 2010, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[43]  D. Goodin The cambridge dictionary of statistics , 1999 .

[44]  C. Herrmann,et al.  Time–Frequency Analysis of Event-Related Potentials: A Brief Tutorial , 2013, Brain Topography.

[45]  Vince D. Calhoun,et al.  Functional and effective connectivity of stopping , 2014, NeuroImage.

[46]  Edward M Bernat,et al.  Externalizing psychopathology and gain-loss feedback in a simulated gambling task: dissociable components of brain response revealed by time-frequency analysis. , 2011, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[47]  P. Michie,et al.  Stop-signal response inhibition in schizophrenia: Behavioural, event-related potential and functional neuroimaging data , 2012, Biological Psychology.

[48]  G. Logan,et al.  On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[49]  E. Basar,et al.  Wavelet analysis of oddball P300. , 2001, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[50]  A. Villringer,et al.  Rolandic alpha and beta EEG rhythms' strengths are inversely related to fMRI‐BOLD signal in primary somatosensory and motor cortex , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[51]  Nicole C. Swann,et al.  Deep Brain Stimulation of the Subthalamic Nucleus Alters the Cortical Profile of Response Inhibition in the Beta Frequency Band: A Scalp EEG Study in Parkinson's Disease , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  E. Donchin,et al.  Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[53]  Kiti Müller,et al.  Relationship of P300 single-trial responses with reaction time and preceding stimulus sequence. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[54]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. , 2012, Annual review of neuroscience.

[55]  Nitin Tandon,et al.  Intracranial electroencephalography reveals different temporal profiles for dorsal- and ventro-lateral prefrontal cortex in preparing to stop action. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[56]  A. Miyake,et al.  The Nature and Organization of Individual Differences in Executive Functions , 2012, Current directions in psychological science.

[57]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[58]  A. Aron The Neural Basis of Inhibition in Cognitive Control , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[59]  A. Aron From Reactive to Proactive and Selective Control: Developing a Richer Model for Stopping Inappropriate Responses , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.

[60]  C. Basar-Eroglu,et al.  Event-related delta and theta brain oscillations reflect age-related changes in both a general and a specific neuronal inhibitory mechanism , 2011, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[61]  W. Byblow,et al.  Selective inhibition of movement. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[62]  Maria C. van de Laar,et al.  Lifespan Changes in Global and Selective Stopping and Performance Adjustments , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[63]  Matthijs Vink,et al.  Frontostriatal activity and connectivity increase during proactive inhibition across adolescence and early adulthood , 2014, Human brain mapping.

[64]  T. Demiralp,et al.  P3 and delta band responses in visual oddball paradigm in schizophrenia , 2008, Neuroscience Letters.

[65]  O. Jensen,et al.  Cross-frequency coupling between neuronal oscillations , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[66]  T. Demiralp,et al.  Comparative analysis of event-related potentials during Go/NoGo and CPT: Decomposition of electrophysiological markers of response inhibition and sustained attention , 2006, Brain Research.

[67]  Robert T. Knight,et al.  Electrophysiological Evidence for Different Inhibitory Mechanisms When Stopping or Changing a Planned Response , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[68]  M. W. Molen,et al.  Development of response activation and inhibition in a selective stop-signal task , 2014, Biological Psychology.

[69]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[70]  M. Hallett,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetry of ipsilateral motor cortex activation during unimanual motor tasks: further evidence for motor dominance , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[71]  A. Nambu,et al.  Functional significance of the cortico–subthalamo–pallidal ‘hyperdirect’ pathway , 2002, Neuroscience Research.

[72]  A. Kok On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[73]  Joaquin A. Anguera,et al.  Reconciling the influence of task-set switching and motor inhibition processes on stop signal after-effects , 2013, Front. Psychol..