Modality-Switching in the Simon Task: The Clash of Reference Frames
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] A. Engel,et al. Tactile remapping: from coordinate transformation to integration in sensorimotor processing , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[2] M. Hollins,et al. Perceived intensity of vibrotactile stimuli: the role of mechanoreceptive channels. , 1996, Somatosensory & motor research.
[3] S. Klapp,et al. Anatomic and environmental dimensions of stimulus-response compatibility: implication for theories of memory coding. , 1979, Acta psychologica.
[4] G. d'Ydewalle,et al. Effects of multiple reference points in spatial stimulus-response compatibility. , 1992, Acta psychologica.
[5] Peter König,et al. Irrelevant tactile stimulation biases visual exploration in external coordinates , 2015, Scientific Reports.
[6] B. Hommel. The relationship between stimulus processing and response selection in the Simon task: Evidence for a temporal overlap , 1993 .
[7] Robert W. Proctor,et al. Multiple spatial codes and temporal overlap in choice-reaction tasks , 1996 .
[8] G. Aschersleben,et al. The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[9] Robert W. Proctor,et al. Principles for Designing Interfaces Compatible With Human Information Processing , 2016, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact..
[10] S. Soto-Faraco,et al. Tactile remapping beyond space , 2010, The European journal of neuroscience.
[11] B. Hommel. Effects of irrelevant spatial S-R compatibility depend on stimulus complexity , 1994, Psychological research.
[12] S. Soto-Faraco,et al. Somatosensory saccades reveal the timing of tactile spatial remapping , 2011, Neuropsychologia.
[13] C. Spence,et al. Early Vision Impairs Tactile Perception in the Blind , 2004, Current Biology.
[14] J. Ioannidis. Why Most Published Research Findings Are False , 2005, PLoS medicine.
[15] Elger L Abrahamse,et al. The premotor theory of attention and the Simon effect. , 2011, Acta psychologica.
[16] Gregor Thut,et al. The costs of crossing paths and switching tasks between audition and vision , 2009, Brain and Cognition.
[17] M. Moscovitch,et al. Must egocentric and environmental frames of reference be aligned to produce spatial S-R compatibility effects? , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[18] M. Reiner,et al. Sensory dominance in combinations of audio, visual and haptic stimuli , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.
[19] Andrea M Philipp,et al. The role of preparation and cue-modality in crossmodal task switching. , 2010, Acta psychologica.
[20] E. Maris,et al. Orienting Attention to an Upcoming Tactile Event Involves a Spatially and Temporally Specific Modulation of Sensorimotor Alpha- and Beta-Band Oscillations , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[21] J. Ridley. Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .
[22] Charles Spence,et al. The cognitive and neural correlates of tactile memory. , 2009, Psychological bulletin.
[23] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Effects of spatial attention on directional manual and ocular responses , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.
[24] Iring Koch,et al. Crossmodal attention switching: auditory dominance in temporal discrimination tasks. , 2014, Acta psychologica.
[25] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .
[26] J R Simon,et al. Processing auditory information: interference from an irrelevant cue. , 1969, The Journal of applied psychology.
[27] J R Simon,et al. Auditory S-R compatibility: reaction time as a function of ear-hand correspondence and ear-response-location correspondence. , 1970, Journal of experimental psychology.
[28] I ROCK,et al. Vision and Touch: An Experimentally Created Conflict between the Two Senses , 1964, Science.
[29] S. Monsell,et al. Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .
[30] Ole Jensen,et al. Tactile expectation modulates pre-stimulus β-band oscillations in human sensorimotor cortex , 2010, NeuroImage.
[31] Carlo Umiltà,et al. Attention shifts produce spatial stimulus codes , 1994, Psychological research.
[32] E. Lauber,et al. Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[33] R. Wallace,et al. S-R compatibility and the idea of a response code. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.
[34] Peter Schroeder-Heister,et al. Spatial Coding and Spatio-Anatomical Mapping: Evidence for a Hierarchical Model of Spatial Stimulus-Response Compatibility , 1990 .
[35] F. Colavita. Human sensory dominance , 1974 .
[36] A. Kingstone,et al. Multisensory executive functioning , 2004, Brain and Cognition.
[37] Jean-Marc Sieffermann,et al. Perceived Intensity of Vibrotactile Stimuli: Do Your Clothes Really Matter? , 2016, EuroHaptics.
[38] Iring Koch,et al. Switching attention between modalities: further evidence for visual dominance , 2010, Psychological research.
[39] S. Soto-Faraco,et al. Changing Reference Frames during the Encoding of Tactile Events , 2008, Current Biology.
[40] T. Stoffer. Attentional focussing and spatial stimulus-response compatibility , 1991, Psychological research.
[41] Andrea M Philipp,et al. Control and interference in task switching--a review. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.
[42] S. Monsell. Task switching , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[43] John J. Foxe,et al. Intersensory selective attention and temporal orienting operate in parallel and are instantiated in spatially distinct sensory and motor cortices , 2015, Human brain mapping.
[44] R. Proctor,et al. The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[45] C. Spence,et al. Developmental vision determines the reference frame for the multisensory control of action , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[46] P. Fitts,et al. S-R compatibility: spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. , 1953, Journal of experimental psychology.
[47] H. Egeth,et al. Toward a translational model of Stroop interference , 1985, Memory & cognition.
[48] T Hasbroucq,et al. Preliminary Evidence for Body-Centered Coding of Tactile Motor Events , 1987, Perceptual and motor skills.
[49] C. Spence,et al. Visual dominance and attention: The Colavita effect revisited , 2007, Perception & psychophysics.
[50] L. G. Gawryszewski,et al. What is crossed in crossed-hand effects? , 1986 .
[51] R. Lubbe,et al. FAILURE OF THE EXTENDED CONTINGENT ATTENTIONAL CAPTURE ACCOUNT IN MULTIMODAL SETTINGS , 2006 .
[52] G. Aschersleben,et al. Automatic visual bias of perceived auditory location , 1998 .
[53] M. Posner,et al. Visual dominance: an information-processing account of its origins and significance. , 1976, Psychological review.
[54] R W Proctor,et al. Auditory stimulus-response compatibility: Is there a contribution of stimulus-hand correspondence? , 2000, Psychological research.
[55] S. Kitazawa,et al. Reversal of subjective temporal order due to arm crossing , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[56] G. Tassinari,et al. Influence of spatial stimulus-response compatibility on reaction time of ipsilateral and contralateral hand to lateralized light stimuli. , 1977, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[57] Michael C. Frank,et al. Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science , 2015, Science.
[58] Bernhard Hommel,et al. Attention and spatial stimulus coding in the Simon task: a rejoinder to van der Lubbe and Abrahamse (2010). , 2011, Acta psychologica.
[59] Avishai Henik,et al. In touch with the Simon effect. , 2014, Experimental psychology.
[60] B Hommel,et al. The role of attention for the Simon effect , 1993, Psychological research.
[61] P. Haggard,et al. Dynamic Tuning of Tactile Localization to Body Posture , 2015, Current Biology.
[62] S. Soto-Faraco,et al. Cross‐modal decoupling in temporal attention , 2014, The European journal of neuroscience.
[63] A. Osman,et al. Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.
[64] Carlo Umiltà,et al. Egocentric and relative spatial codes in S-R compatibility , 1987 .
[65] B. Hommel. Spontaneous decay of response-code activation , 1994, Psychological research.
[66] M. McCloskey,et al. Somatotopic representation of location: evidence from the Simon effect. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[67] D G Pelli,et al. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[68] C. Spence,et al. The cost of expecting events in the wrong sensory modality , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[69] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[70] Elger L. Abrahamse,et al. What determines the specificity of conflict adaptation? A review, critical analysis, and proposed synthesis , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[71] S. Kitazawa. Where conscious sensation takes place , 2002, Consciousness and Cognition.
[72] J. Brooks. Why most published research findings are false: Ioannidis JP, Department of Hygiene and Epidemiology, University of Ioannina School of Medicine, Ioannina, Greece , 2008 .
[73] T Hasbroucq,et al. Stimulus-response compatibility and the Simon effect: toward a conceptual clarification. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.