Inductive reasoning influences perception of interspecies disease transmission risk

Zoonoses (diseases that enter the human population via animal contact) are a major global health concern. Because of how zoonoses emerge, understanding human reasoning about the risk factors associated with animal contact is central to combating their spread. However, little is known about the factors that influence perception of these risks. We present an inductive account of zoonosis risk perception, suggesting that it is influenced by beliefs about the range of animals that are able to transmit diseases to each other. In Study 1, we find that participants who endorse higher likelihoods of cross-species disease transmission have stronger intention to report animal bites. In Study 2, adapting real world descriptions of Ebola virus from the WHO and CDC, we find that communications conveying a broader range of animals as susceptible to a disease increase intentions to report animal bites and decrease perceived safety of wild game meat. These findings suggest that cognitive factors may be harnessed to modulate zoonosis risk perception and combat emerging infectious diseases.

[1]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Category-Based Induction , 1990 .

[2]  Mark C. Samuels,et al.  A hypothesis-assessment model of categorical argument strength , 1996, Cognition.

[3]  D. Heymann,et al.  Prevention is better than cure for emerging infectious diseases , 2014, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[4]  Alejandro López The diversity principle in the testing of arguments , 1995, Memory & cognition.

[5]  H. Field,et al.  Bats: Important Reservoir Hosts of Emerging Viruses , 2006, Clinical Microbiology Reviews.

[6]  Kate E. Jones,et al.  Global trends in emerging infectious diseases , 2008, Nature.

[7]  P. Rozin,et al.  The Sympathetic Magical Law of Similarity, Nominal Realism and Neglect of Negatives in Response to Negative Labels , 1990 .

[8]  Jacob Cohen,et al.  A power primer. , 1992, Psychological bulletin.

[9]  Edward B. Royzman,et al.  Negativity Bias, Negativity Dominance, and Contagion , 2001 .

[10]  D. Albarracín,et al.  Forming attitudes that predict future behavior: a meta-analysis of the attitude-behavior relation. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.

[11]  E. Heit,et al.  Relations between premise similarity and inductive strength , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[12]  P. Daszak,et al.  Emerging infectious diseases of wildlife--threats to biodiversity and human health. , 2000, Science.

[13]  A. Falk,et al.  Individual Risk Attitudes: Measurement, Determinants and Behavioral Consequences , 2009 .

[14]  Kristopher J Preacher,et al.  Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models , 2008, Behavior research methods.

[15]  D. Gentner,et al.  Respects for similarity , 1993 .

[16]  C. Nemeroff,et al.  Magical thinking about illness virulence: conceptions of germs from "safe" versus "dangerous" others. , 1995, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[17]  J. Jones,et al.  Emerging infectious diseases: the role of social sciences , 2012, The Lancet.

[18]  J. Rowcliffe,et al.  Characteristics and Risk Perceptions of Ghanaians Potentially Exposed to Bat-Borne Zoonoses through Bushmeat , 2014, EcoHealth.

[19]  M. Zanna,et al.  On the predictive validity of attitudes: The roles of direct experience and confidence1 , 1978 .

[20]  Clark McCauley,et al.  Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors , 1994 .

[21]  C. Budke,et al.  Knowledge and perceptions of dog-associated zoonoses: Brazos County, Texas, USA. , 2010, Preventive veterinary medicine.

[23]  I. Ajzen,et al.  Correspondence Between Health Attitudes and Behavior , 1986 .

[24]  E. Heit,et al.  Inductive reasoning , 1992, Language Computations.