The Relationship between Impulsive Choice and Impulsive Action: A Cross-Species Translational Study

Maladaptive impulsivity is a core symptom in various psychiatric disorders. However, there is only limited evidence available on whether different measures of impulsivity represent largely unrelated aspects or a unitary construct. In a cross-species translational study, thirty rats were trained in impulsive choice (delayed reward task) and impulsive action (five-choice serial reaction time task) paradigms. The correlation between those measures was assessed during baseline performance and after pharmacological manipulations with the psychostimulant amphetamine and the norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor atomoxetine. In parallel, to validate the animal data, 101 human subjects performed analogous measures of impulsive choice (delay discounting task, DDT) and impulsive action (immediate and delayed memory task, IMT/DMT). Moreover, all subjects completed the Stop Signal Task (SST, as an additional measure of impulsive action) and filled out the Barratt impulsiveness scale (BIS-11). Correlations between DDT and IMT/DMT were determined and a principal component analysis was performed on all human measures of impulsivity. In both rats and humans measures of impulsive choice and impulsive action did not correlate. In rats the within-subject pharmacological effects of amphetamine and atomoxetine did not correlate between tasks, suggesting distinct underlying neural correlates. Furthermore, in humans, principal component analysis identified three independent factors: (1) self-reported impulsivity (BIS-11); (2) impulsive action (IMT/DMT and SST); (3) impulsive choice (DDT). This is the first study directly comparing aspects of impulsivity using a cross-species translational approach. The present data reveal the non-unitary nature of impulsivity on a behavioral and pharmacological level. Collectively, this warrants a stronger focus on the relative contribution of distinct forms of impulsivity in psychopathology.

[1]  L. Diergaarde,et al.  Trait Impulsive Choice Predicts Resistance to Extinction and Propensity to Relapse to Cocaine Seeking: A Bidirectional Investigation , 2012, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[2]  L. Vanderschuren,et al.  Dissociable effects of monoamine reuptake inhibitors on distinct forms of impulsive behavior in rats , 2011, Psychopharmacology.

[3]  Ulrich Müller,et al.  Translational Approaches to Frontostriatal Dysfunction in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Using a Computerized Neuropsychological Battery , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.

[4]  Jan Peters,et al.  The neural mechanisms of inter-temporal decision-making: understanding variability , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  T. Robbins,et al.  Impulsivity, Compulsivity, and Top-Down Cognitive Control , 2011, Neuron.

[6]  N. Paterson,et al.  Sub-optimal performance in the 5-choice serial reaction time task in rats was sensitive to methylphenidate, atomoxetine and d-amphetamine, but unaffected by the COMT inhibitor tolcapone , 2011, Neuroscience Research.

[7]  J. Wiskerke,et al.  μ-Opioid Receptors in the Nucleus Accumbens Shell Region Mediate the Effects of Amphetamine on Inhibitory Control But Not Impulsive Choice , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  James MacKillop,et al.  Delayed reward discounting predicts treatment response for heavy drinkers receiving smoking cessation treatment. , 2009, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[9]  G. Pearlson,et al.  Investigating the behavioral and self-report constructs of impulsivity domains using principal component analysis , 2009, Behavioural pharmacology.

[10]  F Gerard Moeller,et al.  Trait impulsivity and response inhibition in antisocial personality disorder. , 2009, Journal of psychiatric research.

[11]  Warren K Bickel,et al.  Congruence of BOLD Response across Intertemporal Choice Conditions: Fictive and Real Money Gains and Losses , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  P. Killeen An additive-utility model of delay discounting. , 2009, Psychological review.

[13]  D. Dougherty,et al.  Distinctions in Behavioral Impulsivity: Implications for Substance Abuse Research , 2009, Addictive disorders & their treatment.

[14]  M. Bellgrove,et al.  Insights into the neural basis of response inhibition from cognitive and clinical neuroscience , 2009, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[15]  T. Robbins,et al.  Behavioural characterisation of high impulsivity on the 5-choice serial reaction time task: Specific deficits in ‘waiting’ versus ‘stopping’ , 2009, Behavioural Brain Research.

[16]  L. Clark,et al.  Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: Review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[17]  T. Robbins,et al.  Neurobehavioral mechanisms of impulsivity: Fronto-striatal systems and functional neurochemistry , 2008, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[18]  John Scott,et al.  Initial nicotine sensitivity in humans as a function of impulsivity , 2008, Psychopharmacology.

[19]  B. Reynolds,et al.  Dimensions of impulsive behavior in adolescents: laboratory behavioral assessments. , 2008, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[20]  L. Vanderschuren,et al.  The neuropharmacology of impulsive behaviour. , 2008, Trends in pharmacological sciences.

[21]  T. Robbins,et al.  Similar Effects of the Selective Noradrenaline Reuptake Inhibitor Atomoxetine on Three Distinct Forms of Impulsivity in the Rat , 2008, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[22]  L. Diergaarde,et al.  Impulsive Choice and Impulsive Action Predict Vulnerability to Distinct Stages of Nicotine Seeking in Rats , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[23]  F. Dellu-Hagedorn,et al.  Dimensional Analysis of ADHD Subtypes in Rats , 2007, Biological Psychiatry.

[24]  G. Logan,et al.  Restraint and Cancellation: Multiple Inhibition Deficits in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder , 2007, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[25]  Young T. Hong,et al.  Nucleus Accumbens D2/3 Receptors Predict Trait Impulsivity and Cocaine Reinforcement , 2007, Science.

[26]  M. Spronk,et al.  Relationship of delay aversion and response inhibition to extinction learning, aggression, and sexual behaviour , 2006, Behavioural Brain Research.

[27]  T. Robbins,et al.  Reflection Impulsivity in Current and Former Substance Users , 2006, Biological Psychiatry.

[28]  T. Robbins,et al.  Behavioral models of impulsivity in relation to ADHD: Translation between clinical and preclinical studies , 2006, Clinical psychology review.

[29]  L. Vanderschuren,et al.  Critical Involvement of Dopaminergic Neurotransmission in Impulsive Decision Making , 2006, Biological Psychiatry.

[30]  L. Vanderschuren,et al.  Behavioral disinhibition requires dopamine receptor activation , 2006, Psychopharmacology.

[31]  Robert A Zucker,et al.  Poor response inhibition as a predictor of problem drinking and illicit drug use in adolescents at risk for alcoholism and other substance use disorders. , 2006, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[32]  Carla H. Lagorio,et al.  Delay discounting of real and hypothetical rewards III: Steady-state assessments, forced-choice trials, and all real rewards , 2005, Behavioural Processes.

[33]  Trevor W Robbins,et al.  Fractionating Impulsivity: Contrasting Effects of Central 5-HT Depletion on Different Measures of Impulsive Behavior , 2004, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[34]  J. Bjork,et al.  Behavioral impulsivity paradigms: a comparison in hospitalized adolescents with disruptive behavior disorders. , 2003, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[35]  S. Lane,et al.  Relationships Among Laboratory and Psychometric Measures of Impulsivity: Implications in Substance Abuse and Dependence , 2003 .

[36]  William Gardner,et al.  Neurobehavioral disinhibition in childhood predicts early age at onset of substance use disorder. , 2003, The American journal of psychiatry.

[37]  D. Dougherty,et al.  Reliability Estimates for the Immediate and Delayed Memory Tasks , 2002, Perceptual and motor skills.

[38]  D. Dougherty,et al.  Immediate and delayed memory tasks: A computerized behavioral measure of memory, attention, and impulsivity , 2002, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[39]  Warren K Bickel,et al.  Within-subject comparison of real and hypothetical money rewards in delay discounting. , 2002, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[40]  E. Barratt,et al.  Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. , 2001, The American journal of psychiatry.

[41]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  The Ecological Validity of Delay Aversion and Response Inhibition as Measures of Impulsivity in AD/HD: A Supplement to the NIMH Multimodal Treatment Study of AD/HD , 2001, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[42]  A. Scheres,et al.  Response execution and inhibition in children with AD/HD and other disruptive disorders: the role of behavioural activation. , 2001, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[43]  T. Robbins,et al.  The effects of d-amphetamine, chlordiazepoxide, α-flupenthixol and behavioural manipulations on choice of signalled and unsignalled delayed reinforcement in rats , 2000, Psychopharmacology.

[44]  S. Mitchell,et al.  Measures of impulsivity in cigarette smokers and non-smokers , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[45]  J. Evenden Varieties of impulsivity , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[46]  S. Mobini,et al.  Theory and method in the quantitative analysis of ”impulsive choice” behaviour: implications for psychopharmacology , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[47]  G. Logan,et al.  Impulsivity and Inhibitory Control , 1997 .

[48]  J. Patton,et al.  Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. , 1995, Journal of clinical psychology.

[49]  G. Logan,et al.  On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[50]  T. Robbins,et al.  Effects of lesions to ascending noradrenergic neurones on performance of a 5-choice serial reaction task in rats; implications for theories of dorsal noradrenergic bundle function based on selective attention and arousal , 1983, Behavioural Brain Research.

[51]  J. Kagan Reflection--impulsivity: the generality and dynamics of conceptual tempo. , 1966, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[52]  J. Richards,et al.  Dimensions of impulsive behavior: Personality and behavioral measures , 2006 .

[53]  T. Robbins,et al.  Amphetamine impairs the discriminative performance of rats with dorsal noradrenergic bundle lesions on a 5-choice serial reaction time task: New evidence for central dopaminergic-noradrenergic interactions , 2004, Psychopharmacology.

[54]  D. Sheehan,et al.  The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. , 1998, The Journal of clinical psychiatry.

[55]  G. Dunbar,et al.  The Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI). A short diagnostic structured interview: reliability and validity according to the CIDI , 1997, European Psychiatry.

[56]  J. E. Mazur An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. , 1987 .