Correlations between Regional Brain Volumes and Memory Performance in Anoxia

This study was aimed at investigating the quantitative relationship between regional brain volumes (hippocampus, amygdala, as well as cerebrum, frontal lobe, parietal lobe, temporal lobe) and performance on anterograde and retrograde memory tests in anoxic patients. We used high-resolution MRI to measure brain volumes in 13 anoxic patients. Neuropsychological testing was conducted contemporaneously with MRI. To control for age and sex, neuroanatomical volume residuals were calculated using regression equations derived from a group of 87 healthy comparison participants. We found that anoxic patients with severe amnesia had hippocampal volumes that were 36% smaller than normal, whereas patients with mild or no amnesia had normal hippocampal volumes. Regional gray matter volumes in severe amnesic anoxics were substantially smaller than expected. Performances on anterograde memory tests were significantly correlated with hippocampal and regional gray matter volume residuals. There was a significant correlation between white matter volume (but not hippocampal volume) and performance on the Visual Retention Test, a multi-dimensional test of cognitive function. There were no significant correlations between neuroanatomical measures and performance on a retrograde memory test. Our results indicate a strong quantitative relationship between performance on anterograde memory tests and hippocampal and regional gray matter volume residuals. Correlations between white matter volume residuals and performance on the VRT were found to be independent of hippocampal volume. Given the strong correlation between hippocampal volume and total gray matter volume residuals, a quantitative, normalized measure of total gray matter volume may provide a good indication of clinical outcome in anoxia. We thank Natalie Denburg, Jon Spradling, and Kathy Jones for help with participant recruitment, and Jocelyn Cole for help processing the images. Research supported by Program Project Grant NINDS NS 19632 and the Mathers Foundation.

[1]  B. Volpe,et al.  Further characterization of patients with amnesia after cardiac arrest , 1986, Neurology.

[2]  H. Damasio,et al.  Validation of Partial Tissue Segmentation of Single-Channel Magnetic Resonance Images of the Brain , 2000, NeuroImage.

[3]  A. Galaburda,et al.  Human Cerebral Cortex: Localization, Parcellation, and Morphometry with Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 1992, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[4]  M. Castillo,et al.  Diffusion-weighted MR imaging of global cerebral anoxia. , 1999, AJNR. American journal of neuroradiology.

[5]  J. Watson,et al.  Neuropsychological and neuropathological sequelae of cerebral anoxia: A critical review , 2000, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[6]  R. J. Frank,et al.  Brainvox: An Interactive, Multimodal Visualization and Analysis System for Neuroanatomical Imaging , 1997, NeuroImage.

[7]  Oliver T Wolf,et al.  MRI volume of the amygdala: a reliable method allowing separation from the hippocampal formation , 1999, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.

[8]  L. Squire,et al.  Ischemic brain damage and memory impairment: A commentary , 1996, Hippocampus.

[9]  E. Bigler,et al.  Corpus callosum atrophy and neuropsychological outcome following carbon monoxide poisoning , 2002 .

[10]  Hanna Damasio,et al.  Sexual dimorphism and asymmetries in the gray–white composition of the human cerebrum , 2003, NeuroImage.

[11]  V. Magnotta,et al.  A Controlled Quantitative MRI Volumetric Investigation of Hippocampal Contributions to Immediate and Delayed Memory Performance , 2003, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[12]  S. Kesler,et al.  Verbal memory deficits associated with fornix atrophy in carbon monoxide poisoning , 2001, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[13]  M. J. Norušis,et al.  SPSS 13.0 Guide to Data Analysis , 2000 .

[14]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Enhancement of MR Images Using Registration for Signal Averaging , 1998, Journal of Computer Assisted Tomography.

[15]  A. Damasio,et al.  Neuropsychologic detection of abnormal mental decline in older persons. , 1985, JAMA.

[16]  B. Volpe,et al.  The characterization of an amnesic syndrome following hypoxic ischemic injury. , 1983, Archives of neurology.

[17]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Rapid Automated Algorithm for Aligning and Reslicing PET Images , 1992, Journal of computer assisted tomography.

[18]  小野 道夫,et al.  Atlas of the Cerebral Sulci , 1990 .

[19]  L. Weaver,et al.  Severe anoxia with and without concomitant brain atrophy and neuropsychological impairments , 1995, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[20]  B. Siesjö,et al.  The density and distribution of ischemic brain injury in the rat following 2–10 min of forebrain ischemia , 2004, Acta Neuropathologica.

[21]  Daniel Tranel,et al.  Knowing “What” and Knowing “When” , 2006, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[22]  E. Bigler,et al.  Anoxic encephalopathy: neuroradiological and neuropsychological findings. , 1988, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[23]  B E Kendall,et al.  Retrograde amnesia and the volume of critical brain structures , 2003, Hippocampus.

[24]  H. Duvernoy The Human Hippocampus: An Atlas of Applied Anatomy , 1988 .

[25]  J Xiong,et al.  Amygdalar and hippocampal volumetry in control participants: differences regarding handedness. , 2001, AJNR. American journal of neuroradiology.

[26]  R. Adolphs,et al.  Verbal and nonverbal emotional memory following unilateral amygdala damage. , 2001, Learning & memory.

[27]  L. Squire,et al.  Human amnesia and the medial temporal region: enduring memory impairment following a bilateral lesion limited to field CA1 of the hippocampus , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[28]  H. Damasio Human Brain Anatomy in Computerized Images , 1995 .

[29]  Daniel Tranel,et al.  Evidence for preserved emotional memory in normal older persons. , 2003, Emotion.

[30]  John S. Allen,et al.  Normal neuroanatomical variation in the human brain: an MRI-volumetric study. , 2002, American journal of physical anthropology.

[31]  G. Krause,et al.  Brain injury by global ischemia and reperfusion , 1993, Neurology.

[32]  D. Amaral,et al.  Three Cases of Enduring Memory Impairment after Bilateral Damage Limited to the Hippocampal Formation , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  Seth Love,et al.  Neuropathology: A Reference Text of CNS Pathology , 1998 .

[34]  N. Stanhope,et al.  Structural MRI volumetric analysis in patients with organic amnesia, 2: correlations with anterograde memory and executive tests in 40 patients , 2001, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[35]  J. Garcìa,et al.  Cerebral white matter is highly vulnerable to ischemia. , 1996, Stroke.

[36]  H. Duvernoy The Human Brain , 1999, Springer Vienna.

[37]  Y. Iwasaki,et al.  Hypoxic brain damage: cortical laminar necrosis and delayed changes in white matter at sequential MR imaging. , 1993, Radiology.

[38]  E. Bigler,et al.  MRI, quantitative MRI, SPECT, and neuropsychological findings following carbon monoxide poisoning. , 1999, Brain injury.

[39]  T. Kadar,et al.  Sub-regional hippocampal vulnerability in various animal models leading to cognitive dysfunction , 1998, Journal of Neural Transmission.

[40]  M. Tsujimura,et al.  MRI demonstration of cortical laminar necrosis and delayed white matter injury in anoxic encephalopathy , 2004, Neuroradiology.

[41]  Ramona O Hopkins,et al.  Successful Recollection of Remote Autobiographical Memories by Amnesic Patients with Medial Temporal Lobe Lesions , 2003, Neuron.

[42]  F. Plum,et al.  Delayed hippocampal damage in humans following cardiorespiratory arrest , 1987, Neurology.

[43]  Joseph A. Maldjian,et al.  MRI identification of early white matter injury in anoxic–ischemic encephalopathy , 2001 .

[44]  M. Ginsberg,et al.  Hypoxic-ischemic leukoencephalopathy in man. , 1976, Archives of neurology.