Prism adaptation and unilateral neglect: Review and analysis

Theory and data from normal prism adaptation are applied toward understanding the ameliorating effects of prism adaptation for left unilateral neglect patients. Neglect is proposed to be, at least in part, a dysfunction in selection of the region of space appropriate for the task at hand. Normally, a task-work space is strategically sized and positioned (calibrated) around the task-relevant objects. Patients show deficits in both strategic abilities: the task-work space is pathologically reduced in size and patients cannot strategically shift its position. Prism adaptation (spatial realignment) ameliorates dysfunctional positioning, but not sizing of the task-work space. Realignment shifts the egocentric coordinates of a sensory-motor reference frame, thereby bringing at least part of the neglected hemispace into the dysfunctional task-work space: prism adaptation substitutes for dysfunctional positioning, but not sizing of a task-work space. However, such amelioration of dysfunctional positioning may enable relearning of strategic processes (calibration), perhaps, even partially restoring the ability to appropriately size the task-space. Investigation of therapeutic prism adaptation requires methods that permit identification of both the calibration dysfunction and ameliorating realignment.

[1]  G. Vallar Spatial hemineglect in humans , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  Sylvie Chokron,et al.  Neglected attention in apparent spatial compression , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  A. Milner,et al.  Prism Adaptation Improves Chronic Visual and Haptic Neglect: A Single Case Study , 2002, Cortex.

[4]  K. Heilman,et al.  Neglect and Related Disorders , 1984, Seminars in neurology.

[5]  H. Pick,et al.  Visual and proprioceptive adaptation to optical displacement of the visual stimulus. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[6]  Giuseppe Vallar,et al.  Modulation of the Neglect Syndrome by Sensory Stimulation , 1997 .

[7]  Jon Driver,et al.  Auditory and multisensory aspects of visuospatial neglect , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[8]  Felice L. Bedford,et al.  Perceptual and cognitive spatial learning. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  Oculomotor adaptation to prisms is not simply a muscle potentiation effect , 1975 .

[10]  Yves Rossetti,et al.  Bottom-up transfer of sensory-motor plasticity to recovery of spatial cognition: visuomotor adaptation and spatial neglect. , 2003, Progress in brain research.

[11]  Michel Imbert,et al.  Length Representation in Normal and Neglect Subjects with Opposite Reading Habits Studied Through a Line Extension Task , 1997, Cortex.

[12]  B Wallace,et al.  Effects on prism adaptation of duration and timing of visual feedback during pointing. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.

[13]  Gordon M. Redding,et al.  Applications of prism adaptation: a tutorial in theory and method , 2005, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[14]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Alternating prism exposure causes dual adaptation and generalization to a novel displacement , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[15]  Scott H. Johnson-Frey Taking action : cognitive neuroscience perspectives on intentional acts , 2003 .

[16]  G. M. Redding,et al.  Cognitive interference in prism adaptation , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.

[17]  Y. Rossetti,et al.  Simulating unilateral neglect in normals using prism adaptation: implications for theory , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  B Craske,et al.  No retinal component in prism adaptation. , 1974, Acta psychologica.

[19]  G. M. Redding,et al.  Sources of “overadditivity” in prism adaptation , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[20]  B Wallace,et al.  Adaptive coordination and alignment of eye and hand. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[21]  G Rizzolatti,et al.  Study of selective reaching and grasping in a patient with unilateral parietal lesion. Dissociated effects of residual spatial neglect. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[22]  B Wallace,et al.  Calibration and Alignment are Separable: Evidence From Prism Adaptation , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.

[23]  G M Redding,et al.  Additivity in adaptation to optical tilt. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  Y. Rossetti,et al.  Prism adaptation to rightward optical deviation improves postural imbalance in left-hemiparetic patients , 2001, Current Biology.

[25]  I. Kohler,et al.  The formation and transformation of the perceptual world. , 1963 .

[26]  Jason B Mattingley,et al.  Effects of prismatic adaptation on judgements of spatial extent in peripersonal and extrapersonal space , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  Tom Manly,et al.  Is motor perseveration in unilateral neglect ‘driven’ by the presence of neglected left-sided stimuli? , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  Elisabetta Làdavas,et al.  Recovery of oculo-motor bias in neglect patients after prism adaptation , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  Y. Rossetti,et al.  Cognitive bias induced by visuo‐motor adaptation to prisms: a simulation of unilateral neglect in normal individuals? , 2000, Neuroreport.

[30]  J. Baizer,et al.  Cerebellar lesions and prism adaptation in macaque monkeys. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  E. Poulton On prediction in skilled movements. , 1957, Psychological bulletin.

[32]  M Jeannerod,et al.  Visuomotor coordination as a dissociable visual function: experimental and clinical evidence. , 1993, Bailliere's clinical neurology.

[33]  Mark Mapstone,et al.  Cerebral hemispheric specialization for spatial attention: spatial distribution of search-related eye fixations in the absence of neglect , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  Poulton Ec On prediction in skilled movements. , 1957 .

[35]  L. K. Canon,et al.  Influence of concurrent and terminal exposure conditions on the nature of perceptual adaptation. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[36]  M. Hallett,et al.  Adaptation to lateral displacement of vision in patients with lesions of the central nervous system , 1983, Neurology.

[37]  S M Ebenholtz,et al.  The Possible Role of Eye-Muscle Potentiation in Several Forms of Prism Adaptation , 1974, Perception.

[38]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  In search for the egocentric reference. A neurophysiological hypothesis , 1984, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  G M Redding,et al.  Effects of homogeneous and variable exposure on magnitude of adaptation to optical tilt. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  B Wallace,et al.  Adaptive mechanisms in perceptual-motor coordination: components of prism adaptation. , 1988, Journal of motor behavior.

[41]  Components of displacement adaptation in acquisition and decay as a function of hand and hall exposure , 1976 .

[42]  B. Wallace,et al.  Perceptual Adaptation with Selective Reductions of Felt Sensation , 1975 .

[43]  J. Stein The representation of egocentric space in the posterior parietal cortex. , 1992, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[44]  L. Jakobson,et al.  Trajectories of reaches to prismatically-displaced targets: evidence for “automatic” visuomotor recalibration , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[45]  John C. Marshall,et al.  Spatial Compression in Visual Neglect: A Case Study , 1991, Cortex.

[46]  G. M. Redding,et al.  Strategie Calibration and Spatial Alignment: A Model From Prism Adaptation , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.

[47]  Howard N. Zelaznik,et al.  Advances in Motor Learning and Control , 1996 .

[48]  D A Wilkinson Visual-motor control loop: a linear system? , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[49]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Handbook of Neuropsychology , 1991 .

[50]  I P Howard,et al.  The Relative Lability of Mobile and Stationary Components in a Visual-Motor Adaptation Task , 1974, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[51]  Hermann von Helmholtz,et al.  Treatise on Physiological Optics , 1962 .

[52]  R. Finke,et al.  The functional equivalence of mental images and errors of movement , 1979, Cognitive Psychology.

[53]  R B Welch,et al.  Evidence for a three-component model of prism adaptation. , 1974, Journal of experimental psychology.

[54]  Jason B. Mattingley,et al.  Motor role of human inferior parietal lobe revealed in unilateral neglect patients , 1998, Nature.

[55]  R. Andersen,et al.  Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements. , 1997, Annual review of neuroscience.

[56]  M. Mon-Williams,et al.  Motor Control and Learning , 2006 .

[57]  G. M. Redding,et al.  Head posture effects in prism adaptation during hallway exposure , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[58]  Peter Thier,et al.  Parietal Lobe Contributions to Orientation in 3D Space , 1997 .

[59]  J. Mattingley,et al.  The cognitive and neural bases of spatial neglect , 2004 .

[60]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Throwing while looking through prisms. I. Focal olivocerebellar lesions impair adaptation. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[61]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Information integration in perception and communication , 1996 .

[62]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[63]  S. Ebenholtz Additivity of aftereffects of maintained head and eye rotations: An alternative to recalibration , 1976 .

[64]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Bi-directional theory approach to integration , 1996 .

[65]  Felice L. Bedford,et al.  Constraints on learning new mappings between perceptual dimensions , 1989 .

[66]  Yves Rossetti,et al.  Dissociated long lasting improvements of straight-ahead pointing and line bisection tasks in two hemineglect patients , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[67]  K R Paap,et al.  Perceptual consequences of potentiation in the extraocular muscles: an alternative explanation for adaptation to wedge prisms. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[68]  Y. Rossetti,et al.  Reducing spatial neglect by visual and other sensory manipulations: non-cognitive (physiological) routes to the rehabilitation of a cognitive disorder , 2002 .

[69]  Y. Rossetti,et al.  Ameliorating neglect with prism adaptation: visuo-manual and visuo-verbal measures , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[70]  B. Craske,et al.  Spatial discordance is a sufficient condition for oculomotor adaptation to prisms: Eye muscle potentiation need not be a factor , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[71]  Ian H. Robertson,et al.  Unilateral Neglect: Clinical and Experimental Studies edited by Ian H. Robertson and John C. Marshall , 1994 .

[72]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  TRAJECTORY FORMATION IN ARM MOVEMENTS: MINIMIZATION PRINCIPLES AND PROCEDURES , 1996 .

[73]  G. M. Redding,et al.  Cognitive Load and Prism Adaptation. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.

[74]  S M Ebenholtz,et al.  Distance adaptation depends upon plasticity in the oculomotor control system , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[75]  B Wallace,et al.  Prism Adaptation During Target Pointing From Visible and Nonvisible Starting Locations. , 1997, Journal of motor behavior.

[76]  R. Welch Perceptual modification : adapting to altered sensory environments / Robert B. Welch , 1978 .

[77]  S. Kitazawa,et al.  Prism Adaptation of Reaching Movements: Specificity for the Velocity of Reaching , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[78]  Dottie M. Clower,et al.  Selective use of perceptual recalibration versus visuomotor skill acquisition. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[79]  F. Meneghello,et al.  Long-lasting amelioration of visuospatial neglect by prism adaptation. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[80]  E. Bisiach,et al.  Mental Representation in Unilateral Neglect and Related Disorders: The Twentieth Bartlett Memorial Lecture , 1993, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[81]  V. Mountcastle,et al.  Posterior parietal association cortex of the monkey: command functions for operations within extrapersonal space. , 1975, Journal of neurophysiology.

[82]  B Wallace,et al.  Components of prism adaptation in terminal and concurrent exposure: Organization of the eye-hand coordination loop , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[83]  Emmanuel Guigon,et al.  A Neural Model of Perceptual-Motor Alignment , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[84]  J. Uhlarik,et al.  Role of cognitive factors on adaptation to prismatic displacement. , 1973, Journal of experimental psychology.

[85]  Felice L. Bedford,et al.  Perceptual and cognitive spatial learning , 1993 .

[86]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Throwing while looking through prisms , 2005 .

[87]  B Wallace,et al.  Additivity in prism adaptation as manifested in intermanual and interocular transfer , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.

[88]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Throwing while looking through prisms. II. Specificity and storage of multiple gaze-throw calibrations. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[89]  J. Mattingley Visuomotor Adaptation to Optical Prisms: A New Cure for Spatial Neglect? , 2002, Cortex.

[90]  Jason B. Mattingley,et al.  Prismatic adaptation reduces biased temporal order judgements in spatial neglect , 2004, Neuroreport.

[91]  Ian P. Howard,et al.  Additivity of components of prismatic adaptation , 1974 .

[92]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Representation of hand position prior to movement and motor variability. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[93]  Benjamin Wallace,et al.  Dual Prism Adaptation: Calibration or Alignment? , 2003, Journal of motor behavior.

[94]  S. Freedman The Neuropsychology of spatially oriented behavior , 1968 .

[95]  Y. Rossetti,et al.  Preserved prism adaptation in bilateral optic ataxia: strategic versus adaptive reaction to prisms , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[96]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Space and attention in parietal cortex. , 1999, Annual review of neuroscience.

[97]  H. P. Bowditch,et al.  A Comparison of Sight and Touch , 1882, The Journal of physiology.

[98]  Alessandro Farnè,et al.  In search of biased egocentric reference frames in neglect , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[99]  Harutune H Mikaellan,et al.  Adaptation to Displaced Hearing: A Non-Proprioceptive Change , 1974 .

[100]  M. A. Goodale,et al.  Eye movements tell only half the story , 2003, Neurology.

[101]  G. M. Redding,et al.  Effects of Pointing Rate and Availability of Visual Feedback on Visual and Proprioceptive Components of Prism Adaptation. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.

[102]  R. Held,et al.  Adaptation of Disarranged Hand-Eye Coordination Contingent upon Re-Afferent Stimulation , 1958 .

[103]  G. M. Redding,et al.  Perceptual-motor coordination and prism adaptation during locomotion: A control for head posture contributions , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.

[104]  Robert E. Dewar Adaptation to displaced vision: Variations on the “prismatic-shaping” technique , 1971 .

[105]  Giuseppe Vallar,et al.  The Cognitive and Neural Bases of Spatial Neglect , 2002 .

[106]  M. Perenin,et al.  Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect , 1998, Nature.

[107]  H. H. Mikaelian Adaptation to rearranged eye-foot coordination , 1970 .

[108]  G. M. Redding,et al.  Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[109]  Benjamin Wallace,et al.  Attention and prism adaptation , 1985, Cognitive Psychology.

[110]  B Wallace,et al.  Effects of movement duration and visual feedback on visual and proprioceptive components of prism adaptation. , 1994, Journal of motor behavior.

[111]  Analyse de la réorganisation des coordinations visuomotrices chez l'homme. , 1972 .

[112]  Edoardo Bisiach,et al.  Line bisection and cognitive plasticity of unilateral neglect of space , 1983, Brain and Cognition.

[113]  K. Heilman,et al.  Prism adaptation treatment of neglect , 2003, Neurology.

[114]  Gordon M. Redding,et al.  Adaptive Spatial Alignment , 1997 .

[115]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Dynamic coordination of body parts during prism adaptation. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[116]  Y. Rossetti,et al.  Improvement of mental imagery after prism exposure in neglect: a case study. , 1998, Behavioural neurology.

[117]  Harutune H. Mikaeliaan Lack of bilateral generalization of adaptation to auditory rearrangement , 1972 .