Electrophysiology of spatial scene analysis: the mismatch negativity (MMN) is sensitive to the ventriloquism illusion
暂无分享,去创建一个
P. Deltenre | C. Colin | A. Soquet | M. Radeau | B. Dachy | B Dachy | P Deltenre | M Radeau | C Colin | A Soquet
[1] O. Longe,et al. Auditory evoked potentials to abrupt pitch and timbre change of complex tones: electrophysiological evidence of 'streaming'? , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[2] R. Cammann. Is there a mismatch negativity (MMN) in visual modality? , 1990, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[3] I. Winkler,et al. Auditory stream segregation processes operate similarly in school-aged children and adults , 2001, Hearing Research.
[4] Mark T. Wallace,et al. Chapter 8 The visually responsive neuron and beyond: multisensory integration in cat and monkey , 1993 .
[5] W. Ritter,et al. Attention affects the organization of auditory input associated with the mismatch negativity system , 1998, Brain Research.
[6] N. Cowan,et al. The Role of Large-Scale Memory Organization in the Mismatch Negativity Event-Related Brain Potential , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[7] Paul Bertelson,et al. The effect of a textured visual field on modality dominance in a ventriloquism situation , 1976 .
[8] R. Uwer,et al. Stability of mismatch negativities in children , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[9] William H. Press,et al. Book-Review - Numerical Recipes in Pascal - the Art of Scientific Computing , 1989 .
[10] D. Holender,et al. Analytic approaches to human cognition , 1992 .
[11] R. Näätänen,et al. Mismatch negativity--the measure for central sound representation accuracy. , 1997, Audiology & neuro-otology.
[12] O. Aaltonen,et al. Practical Issues in the Clinical Application of Mismatch Negativity , 1995, Ear and hearing.
[13] B. Stein,et al. The Merging of the Senses , 1993 .
[14] K. Krauel,et al. Chemosensory event-related potentials (CSERP) as a key to the psychology of odors. , 2000, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[15] D. Guthrie,et al. Significance testing of difference potentials. , 1991, Psychophysiology.
[16] P. Bertelson,et al. Adaptation to auditory-visual discordance and ventriloquism in semirealistic situations , 1977 .
[17] R. Lynn,et al. A secular decline in the strength of Spearman’s g in Japan , 1994 .
[18] R Näätänen,et al. Mismatch negativity--a unique measure of sensory processing in audition. , 1995, The International journal of neuroscience.
[19] W. Ritter,et al. An investigation of the auditory streaming effect using event-related brain potentials. , 1999, Psychophysiology.
[20] P. Bertelson,et al. The After-Effects of Ventriloquism , 1974, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[21] H. Davis,et al. Principles of electric response audiometry. , 1976, The Annals of otology, rhinology, and laryngology.
[22] The dissection of intersensory bias: weighting for Radeau , 1994, Current psychology of cognition = Cahiers de psychologie cognitive : CPC.
[23] C. V. Jackson,et al. Visual Factors in Auditory Localization , 1953 .
[24] E. Schröger,et al. Fast preattentive processing of location: a functional basis for selective listening in humans , 1997, Neuroscience Letters.
[25] B. Stein,et al. Multisensory integration. Neural and behavioral solutions for dealing with stimuli from different sensory modalities. , 1990, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[26] Robert B. Welch,et al. The “ventriloquist effect”: Visual dominance or response bias? , 1975 .
[27] L. K. Canon. Intermodality inconsistency of input and directed attention as determinants of the nature of adaptation. , 1970, Journal of experimental psychology.
[28] Object identity is not a condition but a result of intersensory integration: the case of audiovisual interactions , 2001 .
[29] K. Reinikainen,et al. Event-related brain potential of human newborns to pitch change of an acoustic stimulus. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[30] I. Winkler,et al. Mismatch negativity is unaffected by top-down predictive information , 2001, Neuroreport.
[31] N. Cowan,et al. Two cognitive systems simultaneously prepared for opposite events. , 1999, Psychophysiology.
[32] B. Stein,et al. Spatial factors determine the activity of multisensory neurons in cat superior colliculus , 1986, Brain Research.
[33] P. Bertelson,et al. Cross-modal bias and perceptual fusion with auditory-visual spatial discordance , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.
[34] R. Näätänen. The role of attention in auditory information processing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive function , 1990, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[35] E. Schröger. On the detection of auditory deviations: a pre-attentive activation model. , 1997, Psychophysiology.
[36] L. Schiebinger,et al. Commentary on Risto Naatanen (1990). The role of attention in auditory information processing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive fenctiono BBS 13s201-2888 , 1991 .
[37] N. Cowan. Attention and Memory: An Integrated Framework , 1995 .
[38] P. Leppänen,et al. Mismatch negativity (MMN) as a tool for investigating auditory discrimination and sensory memory in infants and children , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[39] W. Ritter,et al. Predictability of stimulus deviance and the mismatch negativity , 1998, Neuroreport.
[40] I. Winkler,et al. The concept of auditory stimulus representation in cognitive neuroscience. , 1999, Psychological bulletin.
[41] T. Carrell,et al. Mismatch negativity event-related potential elicited by speech stimuli. , 1992, Ear and hearing.
[42] H Hämäläinen,et al. Is the somatosensory N250 related to deviance discrimination or conscious target detection? , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[43] G. J. Thomas. Experimental study of the influence of vision on sound localization. , 1941 .
[44] Csaba Veres,et al. Towards a general law of numerical/object identity. Commentary. Author's reply , 2001 .
[45] a.R.V.,et al. Clinical neurophysiology , 1961, Neurology.
[46] E. Knudsen,et al. Vision calibrates sound localization in developing barn owls , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[47] I. Winkler,et al. Memory prerequisites of mismatch negativity in the auditory event-related potential (ERP). , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[48] D. Javitt,et al. The Mismatch Negativity of Event‐Related Potentials as a Probe of Transient Auditory Memory: A Review , 1995, Ear and hearing.
[49] D. H. Warren,et al. The role of visual-auditory “compellingness” in the ventriloquism effect: Implications for transitivity among the spatial senses , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.
[50] William H. Press,et al. Numerical recipes in C. The art of scientific computing , 1987 .
[51] A Tales,et al. Mismatch negativity in the visual modality. , 1999, Neuroreport.
[52] J. Fodor. The Modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology , 1986 .
[53] Mikko Sams,et al. Absolute Pitch and Event-Related Brain Potentials , 1993 .
[54] O. Reiser,et al. Principles Of Gestalt Psychology , 1936 .
[55] R. Näätänen,et al. Intermodal selective attention. II. Effects of attentional load on processing of auditory and visual stimuli in central space. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[56] K. Koffka. Principles Of Gestalt Psychology , 1936 .
[57] Béatrice de Gelder,et al. Exploring the relation between mcgurk interference and ventriloquism , 1994, ICSLP.
[58] W R Thurlow,et al. Certain Determinants of the “Ventriloquism Effect” , 1973, Perceptual and motor skills.
[59] Cécile Colin,et al. Rules of intersensory integration in spatial scene analysis and speechreading , 2001 .
[60] K. Reinikainen,et al. Mismatch negativity to change in spatial location of an auditory stimulus. , 1989, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[61] R. Näätänen. Attention and brain function , 1992 .
[62] W R Thurlow,et al. Effects of degree of visual association and angle of displacement on the "ventriloquism" effect. , 1973, Perceptual and motor skills.
[63] M. Tervaniemi,et al. Development of a memory trace for a complex sound in the human brain. , 1993, Neuroreport.
[64] E. Schröger,et al. Mismatch response of the human brain to changes in sound location , 1996, Neuroreport.
[65] M. Radeau. Auditory-visual spatial interaction and modularity , 1994, Current psychology of cognition = Cahiers de psychologie cognitive : CPC.
[66] P. Bertelson,et al. The ventriloquist effect does not depend on the direction of deliberate visual attention , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[67] P. Bertelson,et al. The ventriloquist effect does not depend on the direction of automatic visual attention , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[68] Albert S. Bregman,et al. The Auditory Scene. (Book Reviews: Auditory Scene Analysis. The Perceptual Organization of Sound.) , 1990 .
[69] M T Wallace,et al. The visually responsive neuron and beyond: multisensory integration in cat and monkey. , 1993, Progress in brain research.
[70] M A Meredith,et al. Multisensory Integration , 1990 .
[71] K. Alho,et al. Intermodal selective attention. I. Effects on event-related potentials to lateralized auditory and visual stimuli. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[72] J. Driver. Enhancement of selective listening by illusory mislocation of speech sounds due to lip-reading , 1996, Nature.
[73] I. Winkler,et al. Neural representation for the temporal structure of sound patterns. , 1995, Neuroreport.
[74] R. Näätänen. The Mismatch Negativity: A Powerful Tool for Cognitive Neuroscience , 1995, Ear and hearing.
[75] H G Vaughan,et al. Differentiation of negative event-related potentials in an auditory discrimination task. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[76] S. Jones,et al. Memory-dependent auditory evoked potentials to change in the binaural interaction of noise signals. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.