Implications of distracter effects for the organization of eye movements, hand movements, and perception.

The end positions of eye and hand movements were both drawn towards a distracter that was presented nearby the target. They thus showed a so-called global effect. In contrast, perception was not influenced by the presence of a distracter. These results are discussed with regard to the question whether eye, hand, and perception are based on shared or separate target representations and readout triggers. We conclude that separate representations and readout triggers for eye and hand are the most likely case.

[1]  Jukka Hyönä,et al.  The brain's eye : neurobiological and clinical aspects of oculomotor research , 2002 .

[2]  J. Findlay Global visual processing for saccadic eye movements , 1982, Vision Research.

[3]  J Duncan,et al.  Responses of neurons in macaque area V4 during memory-guided visual search. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[4]  W. Becker,et al.  An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli , 1979, Vision Research.

[5]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[6]  Uta Sailer,et al.  Global effect of a nearby distractor on targeting eye and hand movements. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  Mary Hayhoe,et al.  Visual short-term memory and motor planning. , 2002, Progress in brain research.

[8]  M. Hayhoe,et al.  The coordination of eye, head, and hand movements in a natural task , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  D. Carey,et al.  Neuropsychological perspectives on eye-hand coordination in visually-guided reaching. , 2002, Progress in brain research.

[10]  D Lee,et al.  Effects of exogenous and endogenous attention on visually guided hand movements. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[11]  J M Findlay,et al.  Spatial Scale and Saccade Programming , 1997, Perception.

[12]  J. Duncan Converging levels of analysis in the cognitive neuroscience of visual attention. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[13]  C. C. A. M. Gielen,et al.  Conditions determining early modification of motor programmes in response to changes in target location , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  R. Watt Scanning from coarse to fine spatial scales in the human visual system after the onset of a stimulus. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[15]  Christian Coëffé,et al.  Reducing the influence of non-target stimuli on saccade accuracy: Predictability and latency effects , 1987, Vision Research.

[16]  K. Hoffmann,et al.  Neurons in the primate superior colliculus coding for arm movements in gaze-related coordinates. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Ocular gaze is anchored to the target of an ongoing pointing movement. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Activity of superior colliculus in behaving monkey. II. Effect of attention on neuronal responses. , 1972, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  K. Hoffmann,et al.  Anatomical distribution of arm-movement-related neurons in the primate superior colliculus and underlying reticular formation in comparison with visual and saccadic cells , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  C. C. A. M. Gielen,et al.  Coordination of fast eye and arm movements in a tracking task , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  S. Tipper,et al.  Selective Reaching to Grasp: Evidence for Distractor Interference Effects , 1997 .

[22]  Aitsebaomo Ap,et al.  Saccadic and psychophysical discrimination of double targets. , 2000 .

[23]  Jeremiah Y. Cohen,et al.  The neural basis of saccade target selection , 1995 .

[24]  F. Ottes,et al.  Latency dependence of colour-based target vs nontarget discrimination by the saccadic system , 1985, Vision Research.