Direct visuomotor mapping for fast visually-evoked arm movements

In contrast to conventional reaction time (RT) tasks, saccadic RT's to visual targets are very fast and unaffected by the number of possible targets. This can be explained by the sub-cortical circuitry underlying eye movements, which involves direct mapping between retinal input and motor output in the superior colliculus. Here we asked if the choice-invariance established for the eyes also applies to a special class of fast visuomotor responses of the upper limb. Using a target-pointing paradigm we observed very fast reaction times (<150 ms) which were completely unaffected as the number of possible target choices was increased from 1 to 4. When we introduced a condition of altered stimulus-response mapping, RT went up and a cost of choice was observed. These results can be explained by direct mapping between visual input and motor output, compatible with a sub-cortical pathway for visual control of the upper limb.

[1]  B. Day,et al.  Voluntary modification of automatic arm movements evoked by motion of a visual target , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  Kestutis Kveraga,et al.  Directional Uncertainty in Visually Guided Pointing , 2006, Perceptual and motor skills.

[3]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  A lesion of the posterior parietal cortex disrupts on-line adjustments during aiming movements , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  W. Werner,et al.  Neurons in the Primate Superior Colliculus are Active Before and During Arm Movements to Visual Targets , 1993, The European journal of neuroscience.

[5]  P. Brown,et al.  Evidence for subcortical involvement in the visual control of human reaching. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  J. E. Albano,et al.  Visual-motor function of the primate superior colliculus. , 1980, Annual review of neuroscience.

[7]  M G Saslow,et al.  Latency for saccadic eye movement. , 1967, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[8]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Role of the posterior parietal cortex in updating reaching movements to a visual target , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  H. Hughes,et al.  Smooth pursuit under stimulus-response uncertainty. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[10]  L. Carlton Processing visual feedback information for movement control. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  Marco Schieppati,et al.  The Hoffmann reflex: A means of assessing spinal reflex excitability and its descending control in man , 1987, Progress in Neurobiology.

[12]  J. Soechting,et al.  Modification of trajectory of a pointing movement in response to a change in target location. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  W. E. Hick Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , 1948, Nature.

[14]  Raymond F Reynolds,et al.  Fast visuomotor processing made faster by sound , 2007, The Journal of physiology.

[15]  J Ashe,et al.  Choice and stimulus-response compatibility affect duration of response selection. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[16]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.

[17]  N. J. Gandhi,et al.  Motor functions of the superior colliculus. , 2011, Annual review of neuroscience.

[18]  Kestutis Kveraga,et al.  Effects of Directional Uncertainty on Visually-Guided Joystick Pointing , 2005, Perceptual and motor skills.

[19]  Charles Chubb,et al.  Visually guided, aimed movements can be unaffected by stimulus–response uncertainty , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  B. Fischer,et al.  Human express saccades: extremely short reaction times of goal directed eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  E. Brenner,et al.  Fast Responses of the Human Hand to Changes in Target Position. , 1997, Journal of motor behavior.

[22]  D. Pélisson,et al.  Direct evidence for the contribution of the superior colliculus in the control of visually guided reaching movements in the cat , 2004, The Journal of physiology.

[23]  B. Day,et al.  Rapid visuo-motor processes drive the leg regardless of balance constraints , 2005, Current Biology.

[24]  M. Desmurget,et al.  An ‘automatic pilot’ for the hand in human posterior parietal cortex: toward reinterpreting optic ataxia , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[25]  J. A. Pruszynski,et al.  Stimulus‐locked responses on human arm muscles reveal a rapid neural pathway linking visual input to arm motor output , 2010, The European journal of neuroscience.

[26]  D P Munoz,et al.  Role of Primate Superior Colliculus in Preparation and Execution of Anti-Saccades and Pro-Saccades , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  M. Glickstein Subcortical projections of the parietal lobes. , 2003, Advances in neurology.

[28]  R. Proctor,et al.  Stimulus-Response Compatibility Principles: Data, Theory, and Application , 2006 .

[29]  Kestutis Kveraga,et al.  Effects of stimulus-response uncertainty on saccades to near-threshold targets , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  R. Hyman Stimulus information as a determinant of reaction time. , 1953, Journal of experimental psychology.

[31]  Leanne Boucher,et al.  Saccades operate in violation of Hick’s law , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  Shin'ya Nishida,et al.  Large-Field Visual Motion Directly Induces an Involuntary Rapid Manual Following Response , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.