The joint development of hemispheric lateralization for words and faces.

Consistent with long-standing findings from behavioral studies, neuroimaging investigations have identified a region of the inferior temporal cortex that, in adults, shows greater face selectivity in the right than left hemisphere and, conversely, a region that shows greater word selectivity in the left than right hemisphere. What has not been determined is how this pattern of mature hemispheric specialization emerges over the course of development. The present study examines the hemispheric superiority for faces and words in children, young adolescents and adults in a discrimination task in which stimuli are presented briefly in either hemifield. Whereas adults showed the expected left and right visual field superiority for face and word discrimination, respectively, the young adolescents demonstrated only the right-field superiority for words and no field superiority for faces. Although the children's overall accuracy was lower than that of the older groups, like the young adolescents, they exhibited a right visual field superiority for words but no field superiority for faces. Interestingly, the emergence of face lateralization was correlated with reading competence, measured on an independent standardized test, after regressing out age, quantitative reasoning scores, and face discrimination accuracy. Taken together, these findings suggest that the hemispheric organization of face and word recognition do not develop independently and that word lateralization, which emerges earlier, may drive later face lateralization. A theoretical account in which competition for visual representations unfolds over the course of development is proposed to account for the findings.

[1]  Daniel Brandeis,et al.  Emerging Neurophysiological Specialization for Letter Strings , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[2]  Patrik Vuilleumier,et al.  Differential development of selectivity for faces and bodies in the fusiform gyrus. , 2009, Developmental science.

[3]  T. Allison,et al.  Electrophysiological studies of human face perception. I: Potentials generated in occipitotemporal cortex by face and non-face stimuli. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[4]  Jane E Joseph,et al.  Progressive and regressive developmental changes in neural substrates for face processing: testing specific predictions of the Interactive Specialization account. , 2011, Developmental science.

[5]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  Cathy J. Price,et al.  Top-down modulation of ventral occipito-temporal responses during visual word recognition , 2011, NeuroImage.

[7]  J. Iaccino,et al.  Left Brain - Right Brain Differences: Inquiries, Evidence, and New Approaches , 1993 .

[8]  Martha J. Farah,et al.  The Cognitive Neuroscience of Vision , 2000 .

[9]  Beatriz Luna,et al.  Visual category-selectivity for faces, places and objects emerges along different developmental trajectories. , 2007, Developmental science.

[10]  J. Sergent,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of face and object processing. A positron emission tomography study. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  Development of neural systems for reading. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[12]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The unique role of the visual word form area in reading , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[13]  Marlene Behrmann,et al.  Facing changes and changing faces in adolescence: A new model for investigating adolescent-specific interactions between pubertal, brain and behavioral development , 2012, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  Bruno Rossion,et al.  Category Specificity in Early Perception: Face and Word N170 Responses Differ in Both Lateralization and Habituation Properties , 2008, Frontiers in human neuroscience.

[15]  Andrew N. Meltzoff,et al.  Brain Activation during Face Perception: Evidence of a Developmental Change , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[16]  N. Minshew,et al.  Lack of developmental improvement on a face memory task during adolescence in autism , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  D. Maurer,et al.  Expert face processing requires visual input to the right hemisphere during infancy , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  J E Joseph,et al.  Developmental shifts in cortical loci for face and object recognition , 2004, Neuroreport.

[19]  S. de Schonen,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetry in a face discrimination task in infants. , 1990, Child development.

[20]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Specialization within the ventral stream: the case for the visual word form area , 2004, NeuroImage.

[21]  J Sergent,et al.  Functional and anatomical decomposition of face processing: evidence from prosopagnosia and PET study of normal subjects. , 1992, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[22]  Patricia A. McMullen,et al.  Converging methods for understanding reading and dyslexia , 1999 .

[23]  D. Plaut,et al.  Complementary neural representations for faces and words: A computational exploration , 2011, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[24]  Richard B. Ivry,et al.  Hemispheric Asymmetries , 2000, Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences.

[25]  G. Eden,et al.  Development of Ventral Stream Representations for Single Letters , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[26]  Paul M Matthews,et al.  The Role of the Posterior Fusiform Gyrus in Reading , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Visual word processing and experiential origins of functional selectivity in human extrastriate cortex , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[28]  N. Kanwisher Domain specificity in face perception , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[29]  R. C. Oldfield THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .

[30]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Where cognitive development and aging meet: Face learning ability peaks after age 30 , 2011, Cognition.

[31]  B. Gelder,et al.  Face processing in adolescents with autistic disorder: The inversion and composite effects , 2003, Brain and Cognition.

[32]  S. Carey,et al.  Developmental changes in the representation of faces. , 1977, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[33]  Kristina M. Visscher,et al.  Functional Neuroanatomical Differences Between Adults and School-Age Children in the Processing of Single Words , 2002, Science.

[34]  R. Buxton,et al.  The development of face and location processing: an fMRI study , 2003 .

[35]  C. Price,et al.  The Interactive Account of ventral occipitotemporal contributions to reading , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[36]  Daphne Maurer,et al.  Discrimination of Facial Features by Adults, 10-Year-Olds, and Cataract-Reversal Patients , 2010, Perception.

[37]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  Differential development of high-level visual cortex correlates with category-specific recognition memory , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[38]  Bruno Rossion,et al.  Early lateralization and orientation tuning for face, word, and object processing in the visual cortex , 2003, NeuroImage.

[39]  Cathy J. Price,et al.  Explaining Left Lateralization for Words in the Ventral Occipitotemporal Cortex , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  S. de Schonen,et al.  Early brain lesions and face-processing development. , 2005, Developmental psychobiology.

[41]  R. Rosenfeld Patients , 2012, Otolaryngology--head and neck surgery : official journal of American Academy of Otolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery.

[42]  H. Ellis,et al.  The effects of age and sex upon adolescents' recognition of faces. , 1973, The Journal of genetic psychology.

[43]  Cindy M. Bukach,et al.  Holistic Processing of Words Modulated by Reading Experience , 2011, PloS one.

[44]  A. Toga,et al.  Mapping brain asymmetry , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[45]  Joseph B. Hellige,et al.  Processing Asymmetries in the Visual System , 2010 .

[46]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Neural Basis of the Behavioral Face-Inversion Effect , 2005, Current Biology.

[47]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  C. B. Cave,et al.  Evidence for two types of spatial representations: hemispheric specialization for categorical and coordinate relations. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[49]  Flavio M. de Paula,et al.  Developmental differences in the neural bases of the face inversion effect show progressive tuning of face-selective regions to the upright orientation , 2007, NeuroImage.

[50]  W. Krieg Functional Neuroanatomy , 1953, Springer Series in Experimental Entomology.

[51]  Frederic Dick,et al.  Developmental Changes in Effective Connectivity in the Emerging Core Face Network , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[52]  V. Goffaux,et al.  Newborns’ face recognition is based on spatial frequencies below 0.5 cycles per degree , 2008, Cognition.

[53]  Dianne Phippard Henifield differences in visual perception in deaf and hearing subjects , 1977, Neuropsychologia.

[54]  D. Maurer,et al.  Why 8-year-olds cannot tell the difference between Steve Martin and Paul Newman: factors contributing to the slow development of sensitivity to the spacing of facial features. , 2004, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[55]  Talma Hendler,et al.  Eccentricity Bias as an Organizing Principle for Human High-Order Object Areas , 2002, Neuron.

[56]  T. Allison,et al.  Differential Sensitivity of Human Visual Cortex to Faces, Letterstrings, and Textures: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[57]  Kenneth Hugdahl,et al.  The two halves of the brain : information processing in the cerebral hemispheres , 2010 .

[58]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Location and spatial profile of category‐specific regions in human extrastriate cortex , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[59]  Talma Hendler,et al.  Center–periphery organization of human object areas , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[60]  Daniel Mills,et al.  Left gaze bias in humans, rhesus monkeys and domestic dogs , 2009, Animal Cognition.

[61]  L. Katz,et al.  Laterality and reading proficiency. , 1974, Neuropsychologia.

[62]  J. Cronly-Dillon,et al.  Visual Agnosias and Other Disturbances of Visual Perception and Cognition , 1991 .

[63]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  Fast, visual specialization for reading in English revealed by the topography of the N170 ERP response , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[64]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  The N170 Shows Differential Repetition Effects for Faces, Objects, and Orthographic Stimuli , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[65]  Ferath Kherif,et al.  Automatic Top-Down Processing Explains Common Left Occipito-Temporal Responses to Visual Words and Objects , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[66]  J. Werker,et al.  Infants' ability to match dynamic phonetic and gender information in the face and voice. , 2002, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[67]  S Lehéricy,et al.  The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[68]  S. Dehaene,et al.  How Learning to Read Changes the Cortical Networks for Vision and Language , 2010, Science.

[69]  R. Flin,et al.  Development of face recognition: an encoding switch? , 1985, British journal of psychology.

[70]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Cortical representations of symbols, objects, and faces are pruned back during early childhood. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.