Contextualizing action observation in the predictive brain: Causal contributions of prefrontal and middle temporal areas

&NA; Context facilitates the recognition of forthcoming actions by pointing to which intention is likely to drive them. This intention is thought to be estimated in a ventral pathway linking MTG with frontal regions and to further impact on the implementation of sensory predictions within the action observation network (AON). Additionally, when conflicting intentions are estimated from context, the DLPFC may bias action selection. However, direct evidence for the contribution of these areas to context‐embedded action representations in the AON is still lacking. Here, we used a perturb‐and‐measure TMS‐approach to disrupt neural activity, separately in MTG and DLPFC and subsequently measure cortico‐spinal excitability while observing actions embedded in congruent, incongruent or ambiguous contexts. Context congruency was manipulated in terms of compatibility between observed kinematics and the action goal suggested by the ensemble of objects depicted in the environment. In the control session (vertex), we found an early facilitation and later inhibition for kinematics embedded in congruent and incongruent contexts, respectively. MTG stimulation altered the differential modulation of M1 response to congruent vs. incongruent contexts, suggesting this area specifies prior representations about appropriate object graspability. Interestingly, all effects were abolished after DLPFC stimulation highlighting its critical role in broader contextual modulation of the AON activity. HighlightscTBS to pMTG and DLPFC differently alters M1 responses during action observation.pMTG is involved in coding objects' appropriate graspability.DLPFC mediates top‐down contextual predictions in M1.

[1]  C. Urgesi,et al.  Dissociated Representations of Deceptive Intentions and Kinematic Adaptations in the Observer's Motor System , 2018, Cerebral cortex.

[2]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control , 1996, Neural Networks.

[3]  S. Boniface,et al.  Magnetic brain stimulation with a double coil: the importance of coil orientation. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[4]  J. Osborne Notes on the use of data transformations. , 2002 .

[5]  Alvaro Pascual-Leone,et al.  Reproducibility of the effects of theta burst stimulation on motor cortical plasticity in healthy participants , 2014, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[6]  C. Keysers,et al.  Primary somatosensory contribution to action observation brain activity—combining fMRI and cTBS , 2016, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[7]  P. Dechent,et al.  Is the human primary motor cortex involved in motor imagery? , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[8]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Interindividual variability in response to continuous theta-burst stimulation in healthy adults , 2017, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[10]  Valeria Gazzola,et al.  Primary somatosensory cortex necessary for the perception of weight from other people's action: A continuous theta-burst TMS experiment , 2017, NeuroImage.

[11]  J. Rothwell,et al.  Theta Burst Stimulation of the Human Motor Cortex , 2005, Neuron.

[12]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  Antígona Martínez,et al.  Contributions of low and high spatial frequency processing to impaired object recognition circuitry in schizophrenia. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[14]  C. Padoa-Schioppa,et al.  Contributions of Orbitofrontal and Lateral Prefrontal Cortices to Economic Choice and the Good-to-Action Transformation , 2014, Neuron.

[15]  Salvatore Maria Aglioti,et al.  Harm avoiders suppress motor resonance to observed immoral actions. , 2015, Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience.

[16]  James M. Kilner,et al.  More than one pathway to action understanding , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  Jody C. Culham,et al.  Observing Learned Object-specific Functional Grasps Preferentially Activates the Ventral Stream , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  Cosimo Urgesi,et al.  Compensatory plasticity in the action observation network: virtual lesions of STS enhance anticipatory simulation of seen actions. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[19]  J C Rothwell,et al.  Comparison of descending volleys evoked by transcranial magnetic and electric stimulation in conscious humans. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[20]  Cosimo Urgesi,et al.  Tracking the Time Course of Top-Down Contextual Effects on Motor Responses during Action Comprehension , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  Gregor Thut,et al.  Preperceptual and Stimulus-Selective Enhancement of Low-Level Human Visual Cortex Excitability by Sounds , 2009, Current Biology.

[22]  Maurizio Gentilucci,et al.  Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation of Broca's Area Affects Verbal Responses to Gesture Observation , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[23]  P. Downing,et al.  The lateral occipitotemporal cortex in action , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  Paul Cisek,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of action selection: the affordance competition hypothesis , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[25]  C. Urgesi,et al.  Gender differences in the neural underpinning of perceiving and appreciating the beauty of the body , 2014, Behavioural Brain Research.

[26]  U. Mosimann,et al.  Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex affects divided attention immediately after cessation of stimulation. , 2006, Journal of psychiatric research.

[27]  S. Rossi,et al.  Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research , 2009, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[28]  P. Holmes,et al.  Reflecting on Mirror Mechanisms: Motor Resonance Effects during Action Observation Only Present with Low-Intensity Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation , 2013, PloS one.

[29]  Moritz F. Wurm,et al.  Objects tell us what action we can expect: dissociating brain areas for retrieval and exploitation of action knowledge during action observation in fMRI , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[30]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Motor prediction , 2001, Current Biology.

[31]  J. A. Pruszynski,et al.  Neural correlates , 2023 .

[32]  Scott T. Grafton Embodied Cognition and the Simulation of Action to Understand Others , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[33]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Phase-specific modulation of cortical motor output during movement observation , 2001, Neuroreport.

[34]  M. Bar,et al.  Top-down predictions in the cognitive brain , 2007, Brain and Cognition.

[35]  Antonia F. de C. Hamilton,et al.  Action Understanding Requires the Left Inferior Frontal Cortex , 2006, Current Biology.

[36]  F. Baldissera,et al.  Cyclic time course of motor excitability modulation during the observation of a cyclic hand movement , 2005, Brain Research.

[37]  Robert J. Zatorre,et al.  The Role of the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex in Bimodal Divided Attention: Two Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Studies , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[38]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Safety of Theta Burst Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation: A Systematic Review of the Literature , 2011, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[39]  Nicholas P. Holmes,et al.  Effects of action observation on corticospinal excitability: Muscle specificity, direction, and timing of the mirror response , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  Seppo Kähkönen,et al.  The novelty value of the combined use of electroencephalography and transcranial magnetic stimulation for neuroscience research , 2006, Brain Research Reviews.

[41]  G. Orban,et al.  Neural mechanisms of understanding rational actions: middle temporal gyrus activation by contextual violation. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[42]  Paul B. Fitzgerald,et al.  Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) of the inferior frontal gyrus disrupts interpersonal motor resonance , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  T. Shallice,et al.  Naming manipulable objects: Anatomy of a category specific effect in left temporal tumours , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  Stephan P. Swinnen,et al.  Is the human primary motor cortex activated by muscular or direction-dependent features of observed movements? , 2009, Cortex.

[45]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Evidence for a distributed hierarchy of action representation in the brain. , 2007, Human movement science.

[46]  Kadharbatcha S Saleem,et al.  Subdivisions and connectional networks of the lateral prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkey , 2014, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[47]  Pierre-Alexandre Klein,et al.  Top-down suppression of incompatible motor activations during response selection under conflict , 2014, NeuroImage.

[48]  A. Seth,et al.  Predictions shape confidence in right inferior frontal gyrus , 2016, bioRxiv.

[49]  C. Umilta,et al.  The use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in cognitive neuroscience: A new synthesis of methodological issues , 2011, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[50]  M. Hallett,et al.  Optimal Focal Transcranial Magnetic Activation of the Human Motor Cortex: Effects of Coil Orientation, Shape of the Induced Current Pulse, and Stimulus Intensity , 1992, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[51]  M. Bar,et al.  Magnocellular Projections as the Trigger of Top-Down Facilitation in Recognition , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  H Stanislaw,et al.  Calculation of signal detection theory measures , 1999, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[53]  C. Urgesi,et al.  Simulating the future of actions in the human corticospinal system. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[54]  P. Rossini,et al.  Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord and roots: basic principles and procedures for routine clinical application. Report of an IFCN committee. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[55]  M. Ridding,et al.  Inter- and intra-subject variability of motor cortex plasticity following continuous theta-burst stimulation , 2015, Neuroscience.

[56]  Elizabeth Jefferies,et al.  Going beyond Inferior Prefrontal Involvement in Semantic Control: Evidence for the Additional Contribution of Dorsal Angular Gyrus and Posterior Middle Temporal Cortex , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[57]  Alessandro Livi,et al.  Mirror Neuron Activation Prior to Action Observation in a Predictable Context , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[58]  Marta Kutas,et al.  Tracking the Time Course of Meaning Activation , 1988 .

[59]  Michela Romani,et al.  Motor facilitation during action observation: topographic mapping of the target muscle and influence of the onlooker's posture , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[60]  J. Jonides,et al.  Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[61]  Luigi Cattaneo,et al.  Early and late motor responses to action observation. , 2013, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[62]  E. Halgren,et al.  Top-down facilitation of visual recognition. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[63]  E. Hoshi,et al.  Multisynaptic projections from the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex to the dorsal premotor cortex in macaques – anatomical substrate for conditional visuomotor behavior , 2012, The European journal of neuroscience.

[64]  Cosimo Urgesi,et al.  Contextual modulation of motor resonance during the observation of everyday actions , 2016, NeuroImage.

[65]  Alex Martin,et al.  Grounding Object Concepts in Perception and Action: Evidence from FMRI Studies of Tools , 2007, Cortex.

[66]  M. Raichle,et al.  Localization of a human system for sustained attention by positron emission tomography , 1991, Nature.

[67]  E. DeYoe,et al.  Distinct Cortical Pathways for Processing Tool versus Animal Sounds , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[68]  Theodore P. Zanto,et al.  Top-Down Suppression , 2015 .

[69]  T. Kesar,et al.  The use of transcranial magnetic stimulation to evaluate cortical excitability of lower limb musculature: Challenges and opportunities. , 2018, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.

[70]  J. Rothwell,et al.  The role of interneuron networks in driving human motor cortical plasticity. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[71]  Angela R. Laird,et al.  ALE meta-analysis of action observation and imitation in the human brain , 2010, NeuroImage.

[72]  J. Gross,et al.  Sounds Reset Rhythms of Visual Cortex and Corresponding Human Visual Perception , 2012, Current Biology.

[73]  P. Cisek Integrated Neural Processes for Defining Potential Actions and Deciding between Them: A Computational Model , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[74]  E. Wassermann Risk and safety of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation: report and suggested guidelines from the International Workshop on the Safety of Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation, June 5-7, 1996. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[75]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Grasping the Intentions of Others with One's Own Mirror Neuron System , 2005, PLoS biology.

[76]  Franziska M. Korb,et al.  Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Intensities in Cognitive Paradigms , 2011, PloS one.

[77]  N A Macmillan,et al.  Detection theory analysis of group data: estimating sensitivity from average hit and false-alarm rates. , 1985, Psychological bulletin.

[78]  J. Schwarzbach,et al.  Whole-Brain Haemodynamic After-Effects of 1-Hz Magnetic Stimulation of the Posterior Superior Temporal Cortex During Action Observation , 2012, Brain Topography.

[79]  Emmanuele Tidoni,et al.  Action Simulation Plays a Critical Role in Deceptive Action Recognition , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[80]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neural Circuits Involved in the Recognition of Actions Performed by Nonconspecifics: An fMRI Study , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[81]  R. Nebes,et al.  Patterns of Hand Preference in a Student Population , 1975, Cortex.

[82]  Michela Balconi,et al.  The tDCS effect on alpha brain oscillation for correct vs. incorrect object use. The contribution of the left DLPFC , 2012, Neuroscience Letters.

[83]  S. Bestmann,et al.  Time-Dependent Changes in Human Corticospinal Excitability Reveal Value-Based Competition for Action during Decision Processing , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[84]  M. Candidi,et al.  Mapping Implied Body Actions in the Human Motor System , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[85]  R. Knight,et al.  Insights into Human Behavior from Lesions to the Prefrontal Cortex , 2014, Neuron.

[86]  Luigi Cattaneo,et al.  Bottom-up and top-down visuomotor responses to action observation. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[87]  R. Passingham,et al.  Action observation and acquired motor skills: an FMRI study with expert dancers. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[88]  Ryota Kanai,et al.  Predictions Shape Confidence in Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[89]  S. Wise,et al.  Comparison of Strategy Signals in the Dorsolateral and Orbital Prefrontal Cortex , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[90]  Michela Balconi,et al.  Dorsolateral pFC and the Representation of the Incorrect Use of an Object: The Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Effect on N400 for Visual and Linguistic Stimuli , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[91]  Lucía Amoruso,et al.  Beyond Extrastriate Body Area (EBA) and Fusiform Body Area (FBA): Context Integration in the Meaning of Actions , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[92]  Hakwan Lau,et al.  Prior Expectation Modulates the Interaction between Sensory and Prefrontal Regions in the Human Brain , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[93]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge , 1996, Nature.

[94]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Predictive coding: an account of the mirror neuron system , 2007, Cognitive Processing.

[95]  Nadia Bolognini,et al.  Somatic and Motor Components of Action Simulation , 2007, Current Biology.