Processing of auditory deviants with changes in one versus two stimulus dimensions.

Auditory event-related potentials (ERPs) in response to 50-ms tones were recorded from the human scalp. A standard stimulus (p = .88) and three different deviants were randomly presented via earphones. There were two one-dimensional deviants (one frequency and one location deviant) and one two-dimensional deviant, with changes in both frequency and location. In one condition, subjects read a book and ignored the auditory stimuli, whereas in another condition they tried to discriminate deviants from standards. In the ignore condition, the two-dimensional deviant elicited an enhanced mismatch negativity (MMN) as compared with the MMNs elicited by the one-dimensional deviants. The temporal and the topographic distributions of the two-dimensional MMNs could be modeled by adding the one-dimensional MMNs. This activity of the MMNs probably results from the independent activity of separate neural populations generating the frequency and the location MMN. In the attend condition, the deviance-related ERP effects were not additive in the N2b and P3 range, implicating that the neural processes involved in the conscious detection of changes in location and frequency were not independent.

[1]  A Pfefferbaum,et al.  Event-related potentials to time-deviant and pitch-deviant tones. , 1988, Psychophysiology.

[2]  The localization of non-tactile thermal sensations. , 1969, Psychophysiology.

[3]  K. Reinikainen,et al.  Right hemisphere dominance of different mismatch negativities. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[4]  Risto Näätänen,et al.  5 The Orienting Reflex and the N2 Deflection of the Event-Related Potential (ERP) , 1983 .

[5]  R Näätänen,et al.  Event-related brain potentials in selective listening to frequent and rare stimuli. , 1990, Psychophysiology.

[6]  K. Reinikainen,et al.  Mismatch negativity to change in spatial location of an auditory stimulus. , 1989, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[7]  K. Reinikainen,et al.  Event-related potentials to repetition and change of auditory stimuli. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[8]  K. Alho,et al.  Intermodal selective attention: evidence for processing in tonotopic auditory fields. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[9]  L. McEvoy,et al.  Human auditory cortical mechanisms of sound lateralization: II. Interaural time differences at sound onset , 1993, Hearing Research.

[10]  R A Butler,et al.  The influence of spatial separation of sound sources on the auditory evoked response. , 1972, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Cross-modal selective attention effects on retinal, myogenic, brainstem, and cerebral evoked potentials. , 1990, Psychophysiology.

[12]  K. Alho,et al.  Intermodal selective attention. I. Effects on event-related potentials to lateralized auditory and visual stimuli. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[13]  D Friedman,et al.  A brain event related to the making of a sensory discrimination. , 1979, Science.

[14]  S. Hillyard,et al.  The effects of channel-selective attention on the mismatch negativity wave elicited by deviant tones. , 1991, Psychophysiology.

[15]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Early selective-attention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted. , 1978, Acta psychologica.

[16]  C. C. Wood,et al.  Scalp distributions of event-related potentials: an ambiguity associated with analysis of variance models. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[17]  W. Ritter,et al.  The chronometry of attention-modulated processing and automatic mismatch detection. , 1992, Psychophysiology.

[18]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Mismatch negativity to slight pitch changes outside strong attentional focus , 1993, Biological Psychology.

[19]  H. Semlitsch,et al.  A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[20]  E. N. Sokolov,et al.  Frequency and location specificity of the human vertex N1 wave. , 1988, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[21]  S. Hackley,et al.  An evaluation of the automaticity of sensory processing using event-related potentials and brain-stem reflexes. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[22]  M K Kaiser,et al.  MANOVA method for analyzing repeated measures designs: an extensive primer. , 1985, Psychological bulletin.

[23]  W. Ritter,et al.  The scalp topography of potentials in auditory and visual discrimination tasks. , 1977, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[24]  A Merisalo,et al.  Location vs. frequency of pure tones as a basis of fast discrimination. , 1980, Acta psychologica.

[25]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Auditory frequency discrimination and event-related potentials. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[26]  I. Winkler,et al.  Event-related brain potentials reflect traces of echoic memory in humans , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[27]  Risto N t nen Processing negativity: An evoked-potential reflection. , 1982 .

[28]  Risto Näätänen,et al.  Event-related potentials reveal how non-attended complex sound patterns are represented by the human brain , 1992, Neuroscience Letters.

[29]  R. Näätänen The role of attention in auditory information processing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive function , 1990, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[30]  W. Ritter,et al.  Mismatch detection and the latency of temporal judgements. , 1992, Psychophysiology.

[31]  H G Vaughan,et al.  Differentiation of negative event-related potentials in an auditory discrimination task. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.