Immediate Recall of Driver Warnings in Forward Collision Warning Scenarios

This paper describes driver recall performance for a forward collision warning (FCW) alert immediately after a critical lead vehicle braking event. A sample of 120 younger and older test participants, balanced by gender, participated in a study of alternative FCW alerts in the Ford Motor Company VIRTTEX driving simulator. A baseline group of participants received no FCW support. Each of the other participants experienced one and only one FCW alert type, visual, auditory, or both. Half of the participants who received an FCW alert had been given knowledge that such a system was in the test vehicle, and half of the participants had been told nothing. A digit-reading distraction task was presented repeatedly during the test drive. The last repetition of this task coincided with the lead vehicle braking event and FCW. Immediately after the event, test participants were asked if they had received a warning and, if so, what they recalled about it. Results were analyzed for those 93 warned test participants who interrupted the digit-reading task to respond to the FCW alert. Approximately 26% of these test participants did not remember receiving a warning at all. Only 58% of the test participants who recollected a warning could accurately recall its modality in all its details, although nearly 90% of those who received the combined audio and video warning recalled at least one of the modalities correctly. Those who received FCW information before the drive had significantly greater recollection than those who were not given any FCW a priori information. Age and gender differences were not statistically significant. The implications of these results are discussed.

[1]  R. Klatzky Human Memory: Structures And Processes , 1975 .

[2]  af Wåhlberg On the validity of self-reported traffic accident data , 2002 .

[3]  Thomas A. Dingus,et al.  The 100-Car Naturalistic Driving Study Phase II – Results of the 100-Car Field Experiment , 2006 .

[4]  Elizabeth F. Loftus,et al.  Memory, surprising new insights into how we remember and why we forget , 1980 .

[5]  Larry Cathey,et al.  Motion Characteristics of the VIRTTEX Motion System , 2001 .

[6]  M. C. Smith,et al.  Theories of the psychological refractory period. , 1967, Psychological bulletin.

[7]  Alan E Stewart,et al.  Attributions of responsibility for motor vehicle crashes. , 2005, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[8]  D. Leblanc,et al.  DEVELOPMENT AND VALIDATION OF FUNCTIONAL DEFINITIONS AND EVALUATION PROCEDURES FOR COLLISION WARNING/AVOIDANCE SYSTEMS , 1999 .

[9]  G. Underwood,et al.  Forgetting near-accidents: the roles of severity, culpability and experience in the poor recall of dangerous driving situations , 2000 .

[10]  Elizabeth F. Loftus,et al.  Eyewitness Memory for People and Events , 2003 .

[11]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  The Seven Sins of Memory: How the Mind Forgets and Remembers , 2001 .

[12]  P Chapman,et al.  Distortion of drivers' speed and time estimates in dangerous situations , 2005 .

[13]  Jeffry Allen Greenberg,et al.  Realistic 3D Sound Simulation in the VIRTTEX Driving Simulator , 2003 .

[14]  Jeff Allen Greenberg,et al.  Driver Distraction: Evaluation with Event Detection Paradigm , 2003 .

[15]  E. Loftus,et al.  Reconstruction of automobile destruction: An example of the interaction between language and memory , 1974 .

[16]  A. Baddeley Your Memory: A User's Guide , 1982 .