Reproductive cessation in female mammals

In female mammals, fertility declines abruptly at an advanced age. The human menopause is one example, but reproductive cessation has also been documented in non-human primates, rodents, whales, dogs, rabbits, elephants and domestic livestock. The human menopause has been considered an evolutionary adaptation assuming that elderly women avoid the increasing complications of continued childbirth to better nurture their current children and grandchildren. But an abrupt reproductive decline might be only a non-adaptive by-product of life-history patterns. Because so many individuals die from starvation, disease and predation, detrimental genetic traits can persist (or even be favoured) as long as their deleterious effects are delayed until an advanced age is reached, and, for a given pattern of mortality, there should be an age by which selection would be too weak to prevent the onset of reproductive senescence,,. We provide a systematic test of these alternatives using field data from two species in which grandmothers frequently engage in kin-directed behaviour. Both species show abrupt age-specific changes in reproductive performance that are characteristic of menopause. But elderly females do not suffer increased mortality costs of reproduction, nor do post-reproductive females enhance the fitness of grandchildren or older children. Instead, reproductive cessation appears to result from senescence.

[1]  A. Hurtado,et al.  The evolution of premature reproductive senescence and menopause in human females , 1991, Human nature.

[2]  P. Medawar,et al.  The Uniqueness of the Individual , 1957, The Uniqueness of the Individual.

[3]  P. Holgate,et al.  Matrix Population Models. , 1990 .

[4]  J. D. Neill,et al.  The Physiology of reproduction , 1988 .

[5]  E. T. te Velde,et al.  Age at natural menopause in a population-based screening cohort: the role of menarche, fecundity, and lifestyle factors. , 1997, Fertility and sterility.

[6]  C. Packer,et al.  Reproductive constraints on aggressive competition in female baboons , 1995, Nature.

[7]  C. Packer,et al.  Roaring and numerical assessment in contests between groups of female lions, Panthera leo , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[8]  The Lack Clutch in a Communal Breeder: Lion Litter Size is a Mixed Evolutionarily Stable Strategy , 1995, The American Naturalist.

[9]  C. Packer,et al.  Non-offspring nursing in social carnivores: minimizing the costs , 1994 .

[10]  S. Altmann The Pregnancy Sign in Savannah Baboons , 1973 .

[11]  C. Packer,et al.  Complex cooperative strategies in group-territorial African lions , 1995, Science.

[12]  R. Laws,et al.  Elephants and their habitats , 1975 .

[13]  C. Packer,et al.  Why Lions Form Groups: Food is Not Enough , 1990, The American Naturalist.

[14]  D. Reznick Costs of reproduction: an evaluation of the empirical evidence , 1985 .

[15]  A. Yashin,et al.  Heterogeneity's ruses: some surprising effects of selection on population dynamics. , 1985, The American statistician.

[16]  Elisa T. Lee,et al.  Statistical Methods for Survival Data Analysis , 1994, IEEE Transactions on Reliability.

[17]  Caleb E. Finch,et al.  Longevity, senescence, and the genome , 1990 .

[18]  M. Sauer,et al.  The impact of the woman's age on the success of standard and donor in vitro fertilization. , 1997, Fertility and sterility.

[19]  K. Krajnak,et al.  Menopause: The Aging of Multiple Pacemakers , 1996, Science.

[20]  Brian Charlesworth,et al.  Evolution in Age-Structured Populations. , 1983 .

[21]  George C. Williams,et al.  PLEIOTROPY, NATURAL SELECTION, AND THE EVOLUTION OF SENESCENCE , 1957, Science of Aging Knowledge Environment.

[22]  K. Hawkes,et al.  Hadza Women's Time Allocation, Offspring Provisioning, and the Evolution of Long Postmenopausal Life Spans , 1997, Current Anthropology.

[23]  W. Hamilton The moulding of senescence by natural selection. , 1966, Journal of theoretical biology.