Effects of overshadowing on conditioned and unconditioned nausea in a rotation paradigm with humans

We examine whether overshadowing by salient stimuli is effective in reducing the ability of a certain environment (the putative conditioned stimulus) to evoke conditioned nausea in healthy humans that experience nausea-evoking rotation (as the unconditioned stimulus, US) in that environment. Twenty-four rotation-susceptible subjects (12 males, 12 females) were randomly assigned to receive either overshadowing by salient tasting beverages (OS+), or a control treatment (a familiar beverage, water; OS−) prior to rotation on three consecutive days (acquisition). To control for taste experiences, the alternative beverage was consumed 12 h later in the home environment (OS+: water, OS−: salient beverage). At Day 4 (test), all subjects drank the familiar beverage (water) prior to rotation (US). Rotation was standardized as 2 × 1-min rotation/day. Nausea was determined by a 7-item symptom scale measuring symptom number (SN) prior to (anticipatory), immediately after, and 15 and 30 min after rotation and by the Nausea Profile (NP) questionnaire immediately after rotation. Cortisol and tumour necrosis factor (TNF)-α in saliva were sampled at the same time-points. SN and cortisol were also measured at home. Overshadowing reduced anticipatory (conditioned) SN. Post-rotation nausea (i.e. the unconditioned response) measured by the NP decreased within the OS+ group only. Anticipatory cortisol and TNF-α were not affected by overshadowing. Treatment × gender interactions manifested for post-rotation cortisol and TNF-α. Groups did not differ in SN and cortisol at home. Overshadowing is effective in reducing symptoms of anticipatory nausea and rotation-induced unconditioned nausea; its effect on endocrine and immunological parameters is gender specific. Its application in alleviation of anticipatory nausea in cancer patients is considered.

[1]  T. Shors,et al.  Sex differences in learning processes of classical and operant conditioning , 2009, Physiology & Behavior.

[2]  C. Kirschbaum,et al.  Synthesis of a cortisol-biotin conjugate and evaluation as a tracer in an immunoassay for salivary cortisol measurement , 1992, The Journal of Steroid Biochemistry and Molecular Biology.

[3]  C. Kirschbaum,et al.  Sex-specific adaptation of endocrine and inflammatory responses to repeated nauseogenic body rotation , 2006, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[4]  A. Molassiotis,et al.  Anticipatory nausea and vomiting , 2004, Supportive Care in Cancer.

[5]  R. Rogers,et al.  Tumor Necrosis Factor Potentiates Central Vagal Afferent Signaling by Modulating Ryanodine Channels , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  J. Goldstein,et al.  Estradiol Modulates Medial Prefrontal Cortex and Amygdala Activity During Fear Extinction in Women and Female Rats , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.

[7]  E. Alba,et al.  Anticipatory nausea and vomiting: prevalence and predictors in chemotherapy patients. , 1989, Oncology.

[8]  Geoffrey Hall,et al.  Latent Inhibition of Rotation Chair-Induced Nausea in Healthy Male and Female Volunteers , 2005, Psychosomatic medicine.

[9]  Edgar Erdfelder,et al.  G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences , 2007, Behavior research methods.

[10]  M. Aapro,et al.  The incidence of anticipatory nausea and vomiting after repeat cycle chemotherapy: the effect of granisetron. , 1994, British Journal of Cancer.

[11]  E. Muth,et al.  Cortisol levels predict motion sickness tolerance in women but not in men , 2009, Physiology & Behavior.

[12]  J. Holland,et al.  Prevalence, predictors, and course of anticipatory nausea in women receiving adjuvant chemotherapy for breast cancer , 1988, Cancer.

[13]  T. Shors Learning during stressful times. , 2004, Learning & memory.

[14]  H. Watanobe Sexual dimorphism in the pituitary-adrenal response to tumor necrosis factor-α, but not to interleukin-6, in the rat , 2002, Brain Research Bulletin.

[15]  L. Menditto,et al.  Impact on daily functioning and indirect/direct costs associated with chemotherapy-induced nausea and vomiting (CINV) in a US population , 2011, Supportive Care in Cancer.

[16]  R. Rescorla,et al.  Potentiation rather than overshadowing in flavor-aversion learning: an analysis in terms of within-compound associations. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[17]  F. Schmäl,et al.  Neuronal Mechanisms and the Treatment of Motion Sickness , 2013, Pharmacology.

[18]  G. Morrow,et al.  Progress in reducing nausea and emesis. Comparisons of ondansetron (zofran), granisetron (kytril), and tropisetron (navoban) , 1995, Cancer.

[19]  G. Hall,et al.  Postinjection suppression of drinking is modified by the presence of conditioned contextual cues: Implications for both anticipatory and posttreatment nausea in humans , 2002, Animal learning & behavior.

[20]  N. Spear,et al.  Changes in the retrievability of associations to elements of the compound CS determine the expression of overshadowing , 1990 .

[21]  Jacob Cohen,et al.  A power primer. , 1992, Psychological bulletin.

[22]  I. Bernstein,et al.  Candy as a scapegoat in the prevention of food aversions in children receiving chemotherapy , 1987, Cancer.

[23]  S. Klosterhalfen,et al.  Effects of overshadowing on conditioned nausea in cancer patients: an experimental study , 1998, Physiology & Behavior.

[24]  H. Schmoll,et al.  Chemotherapy-induced nausea and vomiting: current and new standards in the antiemetic prophylaxis and treatment. , 2005, European journal of cancer.

[25]  J F Thayer,et al.  Assessment of the multiple dimensions of nausea: the Nausea Profile (NP). , 1996, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[26]  L. Hansson,et al.  Self-rated Health Is Related to Levels of Circulating Cytokines , 2004, Psychosomatic medicine.

[27]  A. Molassiotis,et al.  Anticipatory nausea and vomiting , 2005, Supportive Care in Cancer.

[28]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[29]  S. Klosterhalfen,et al.  Anticipatory nausea in cancer patients receiving chemotherapy: Classical conditioning etiology and therapeutical implications , 1993, Integrative physiological and behavioral science : the official journal of the Pavlovian Society.

[30]  M. Tejani,et al.  Anticipatory nausea and vomiting due to chemotherapy. , 2014, European journal of pharmacology.

[31]  R. Lubow,et al.  Latent inhibition: the effect of nonreinforced pre-exposure to the conditional stimulus. , 1959, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[32]  B. Otto,et al.  Pavlovian Conditioning of Taste Aversion Using a Motion Sickness Paradigm , 2000, Psychosomatic medicine.

[33]  Overshadowing not potentiation of illness-based contextual conditioning by a novel taste , 1999 .

[34]  P. Enck,et al.  Gender and the nocebo response following conditioning and expectancy. , 2009, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[35]  R. Dantzer Cytokine‐Induced Sickness Behavior: Mechanisms and Implications , 2001, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[36]  B. Yates,et al.  Physiological basis and pharmacology of motion sickness: an update , 1998, Brain Research Bulletin.

[37]  Marion E. Schneider,et al.  Anticipatory Symptoms and Anticipatory Immune Responses in Pediatric Cancer Patients Receiving Chemotherapy: Features of a Classically Conditioned Response? , 2000, Brain, Behavior, and Immunity.

[38]  Interfering with taste aversion learning in rats: The role of associative interference , 1985, Appetite.

[39]  K. Chambers Hormonal influences on sexual dimorphism in rate of extinction of a conditioned taste aversion in rats. , 1976, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[40]  R. Boakes,et al.  Prevalence of anticipatory nausea and other side-effects in cancer patients receiving chemotherapy. , 1993, European journal of cancer.