Relationship between amygdala responses to masked faces and mood state and treatment in major depressive disorder.

CONTEXT Major depressive disorder (MDD) is associated with behavioral and neurophysiological evidence of mood-congruent processing biases toward explicitly presented, emotionally valenced stimuli. However, few studies have investigated such biases toward implicitly presented stimuli. OBJECTIVE To investigate differential amygdala responses to sad, happy, and neutral faces presented below the level of explicit conscious awareness using a backward masking task in unmedicated participants with MDD and healthy controls (HCs). DESIGN Initial cross-sectional design followed by a longitudinal treatment trial using functional magnetic resonance imaging. SETTING Psychiatric outpatient clinic at the National Institute of Mental Health. PARTICIPANTS We studied 22 unmedicated, currently depressed people with MDD (dMDD), 16 unmedicated individuals with MDD in full remission (rMDD), and 25 HCs. INTERVENTION Ten dMDD participants underwent 8 weeks of antidepressant treatment with the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor sertraline hydrochloride. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES Amygdala region-of-interest and whole-brain analyses evaluated the hemodynamic response during exposure to masked sad vs masked happy faces, to masked sad vs neutral faces, and to masked happy vs neutral faces. RESULTS The dMDD participants showed greater amygdala responses than HCs to masked sad faces, whereas HCs showed greater amygdala responses to masked happy faces. The bias toward sad faces also was evident in rMDD participants relative to HCs and did not differ between dMDD and rMDD participants. This processing bias reversed toward the normative pattern in dMDD participants after sertraline treatment. CONCLUSIONS Emotional-processing biases occur in amygdala responses to sad faces presented below the level of conscious awareness in dMDD or rMDD individuals and to happy faces in HCs. By influencing the salience of social stimuli, mood-congruent processing biases in the amygdala may contribute to dysfunction in conscious perceptions and social interactions in MDD. Our data suggest, however, that the negative bias resolves and a positive bias develops in patients with MDD during selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor treatment.

[1]  P. Cowen,et al.  Effect of acute antidepressant administration on negative affective bias in depressed patients. , 2009, The American journal of psychiatry.

[2]  A. Ohman,et al.  Masking the face: recognition of emotional facial expressions as a function of the parameters of backward masking. , 1993, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[3]  S. Rauch,et al.  Masked Presentations of Emotional Facial Expressions Modulate Amygdala Activity without Explicit Knowledge , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  J. Tanaka,et al.  The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants , 2009, Psychiatry Research.

[5]  K. Mogg,et al.  Attentional bias in anxiety and depression: the role of awareness. , 1995, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[6]  P. Cowen,et al.  Short-term antidepressant treatment modulates amygdala response to happy faces , 2009, Psychopharmacology.

[7]  T. Robbins,et al.  Neuropsychological impairment in patients with major depressive disorder: the effects of feedback on task performance , 2003, Psychological Medicine.

[8]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Functional MRI : an introduction to methods , 2002 .

[9]  Pienie Zwitserlood,et al.  Automatic Mood-Congruent Amygdala Responses to Masked Facial Expressions in Major Depression , 2010, Biological Psychiatry.

[10]  M. Hamilton A RATING SCALE FOR DEPRESSION , 1960, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[11]  Peter Fransson,et al.  Keeping It Short , 2004, Psychological science.

[12]  John Suckling,et al.  Attenuation of the neural response to sad faces in major depression by antidepressant treatment: a prospective, event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. , 2004, Archives of general psychiatry.

[13]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. , 1992, Behavioral neuroscience.

[14]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Sensory tuning beyond the sensory system: an initial analysis of auditory response properties of neurons in the lateral amygdaloid nucleus and overlying areas of the striatum , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  T. Robbins,et al.  Emotional bias and inhibitory control processes in mania and depression , 1999, Psychological Medicine.

[16]  Denis Dooley,et al.  Atlas of the Human Brain. , 1971 .

[17]  D. Perrett,et al.  A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions , 1996, Nature.

[18]  C. Spielberger,et al.  Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory , 1970 .

[19]  S. Hollon,et al.  Cognitive self-statements in depression: Development of an automatic thoughts questionnaire , 1980, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[20]  M. Thase,et al.  Can’t shake that feeling: event-related fMRI assessment of sustained amygdala activity in response to emotional information in depressed individuals , 2002, Biological Psychiatry.

[21]  S. Rauch,et al.  Neurobiology of emotion perception I: the neural basis of normal emotion perception , 2003, Biological Psychiatry.

[22]  R. Dolan,et al.  A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating "unseen" fear. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[23]  S. Paradiso The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life , 1998 .

[24]  E. Rolls,et al.  Neurons in the amygdala of the monkey with responses selective for faces , 1985, Behavioural Brain Research.

[25]  A. Young,et al.  A differential pattern of neural response toward sad versus happy facial expressions in major depressive disorder , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.

[26]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Spatial and temporal resolution in fMRI , 2001 .

[27]  Guy B. Williams,et al.  Neural basis of abnormal response to negative feedback in unmedicated mood disorders , 2008, NeuroImage.

[28]  I. Gotlib,et al.  Attentional biases for negative interpersonal stimuli in clinical depression. , 2004, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[29]  Catherine J. Harmer,et al.  Serotonin and emotional processing: Does it help explain antidepressant drug action? , 2008, Neuropharmacology.

[30]  A. Rush,et al.  The Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology (IDS): psychometric properties , 1996, Psychological Medicine.

[31]  M. Mintun,et al.  Increased amygdala response to masked emotional faces in depressed subjects resolves with antidepressant treatment: an fMRI study , 2001, Biological Psychiatry.

[32]  Guy M. Goodwin,et al.  Antidepressant Drug Treatment Modifies the Neural Processing of Nonconscious Threat Cues , 2006, Biological Psychiatry.

[33]  T. Q. Irigaray,et al.  Intellectual abilities in Alzheimer's disease patients: Contributions from the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) , 2010 .

[34]  E. Murray The amygdala, reward and emotion , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[35]  R. Adolphs Neural systems for recognizing emotion , 2002, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[36]  R. Spitzer Dsm-IV Casebook: A Learning Companion to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition , 1994 .

[37]  K. Mogg,et al.  Implicit and explicit memory for emotion-congruent information in clinical depression and anxiety. , 1995, Behaviour research and therapy.

[38]  Andrew W. Young,et al.  Neuropsychology of fear and loathing , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[39]  D. Williamson,et al.  Evidence for attention to threatening stimuli in depression. , 1996, Behaviour research and therapy.

[40]  A. Wells,et al.  The Thought Control Questionnaire: a measure of individual differences in the control of unwanted thoughts. , 1994, Behaviour research and therapy.

[41]  J. Talairach,et al.  Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging , 1988 .

[42]  J. Leppänen Emotional information processing in mood disorders: a review of behavioral and neuroimaging findings , 2006, Current opinion in psychiatry.

[43]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  C. MacLeod,et al.  Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. , 2005, Annual review of clinical psychology.

[45]  Luke Clark,et al.  Mood-congruent bias in affective go/no-go performance of unmedicated patients with major depressive disorder. , 2005, The American journal of psychiatry.

[46]  A. Nierenberg,et al.  Definitions of antidepressant treatment response, remission, nonresponse, partial response, and other relevant outcomes: a focus on treatment-resistant depression. , 2001, The Journal of clinical psychiatry.

[47]  E. Phelps,et al.  Neural mechanisms mediating optimism bias , 2007, Nature.

[48]  L. Alloy,et al.  Mood congruence and depressive deficits in memory: a forced-recall analysis. , 1999, Memory.

[49]  W. Drevets,et al.  Discovering Endophenotypes for Major Depression , 2004, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[50]  Sheri L. Johnson,et al.  Coherence and specificity of information-processing biases in depression and social phobia. , 2004, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[51]  Deborah A Yurgelun-Todd,et al.  Activation of the amygdala and anterior cingulate during nonconscious processing of sad versus happy faces , 2004, NeuroImage.

[52]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[53]  M. Furey,et al.  Brain structural and functional abnormalities in mood disorders: implications for neurocircuitry models of depression , 2008, Brain Structure and Function.