Cognitive motion extrapolation and cognitive clocking in prediction motion task.

An interruption paradigm was used to measure judgments that rely on cognitive extrapolation of approach and lateral motion. In some conditions the pattern of errors was consistent with that obtained with time-to-contact (TTC) judgments measured with a prediction motion (PM) task. Also, the slope of the relationship between estimated and actual TTC in judgments of approaching objects decreased when visual information about the environment between the observer and the display was minimized. Moreover, the accuracy of relative duration judgments of visual (but not auditory) stimuli decreased when a PM task was performed concurrently. Results are consistent with the notion that PM tasks involve cognitive motion extrapolation rather than solely a clocking process that counts down TTC.

[1]  W. Schiff,et al.  Aging persons' estimates of vehicular motion. , 1992, Psychology and aging.

[2]  D. Lyon,et al.  Time course of visual extrapolation accuracy. , 1995, Acta psychologica.

[3]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[4]  P R DeLucia,et al.  Pictorial and motion-based depth information during active control of self-motion: size-arrival effects on collision avoidance. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  R. Shepard,et al.  CHRONOMETRIC STUDIES OF THE ROTATION OF MENTAL IMAGES , 1973 .

[6]  Patricia R. Delucia,et al.  Judgments of relative time-to-contact of more than two approaching objects: Toward a method , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[7]  Gregory Wayne Liddell Interfering and updating cognitive representations used in judgments of absolute time-to-contact in a prediction-motion task , 1997 .

[8]  W. Chase,et al.  Visual information processing. , 1974 .

[9]  C Fortin,et al.  Temporal interval production and processing in working memory , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  S. J. Segal,et al.  Influence of imaged pictures and sounds on detection of visual and auditory signals. , 1970, Journal of experimental psychology.

[11]  S. W. Brown,et al.  Multiple timing and the allocation of attention. , 1990, Acta psychologica.

[12]  Patricia R. Delucia,et al.  Effects of Pictorial Relative Size and Ground-Intercept Information on Judgments about Potential Collisions in Perspective Displays , 1995, Hum. Factors.

[13]  H. Ross,et al.  Optic-Flow and Cognitive Factors in Time-to-Collision Estimates , 1983, Perception.

[14]  M K Kaiser,et al.  Optical specification of time-to-passage: observers' sensitivity to global tau. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  S. S. Stevens,et al.  Handbook of experimental psychology , 1951 .

[16]  B Brown,et al.  Predicting the Future Position of a Moving Target , 1991, Perception.

[17]  M. Treisman,et al.  The Internal Clock: Evidence for a Temporal Oscillator Underlying Time Perception with Some Estimates of its Characteristic Frequency , 1990, Perception.

[18]  Richard A. Block,et al.  Cognitive models of psychological time , 1992 .

[19]  P R DeLucia,et al.  Pictorial and motion-based information for depth perception. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  L. Kaufman,et al.  Handbook of perception and human performance , 1986 .

[21]  R J Jagacinski,et al.  Quantifying the cognitive trajectories of extrapolated movements. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  A Faulkner,et al.  On the Relation between Time Perception and the Timing of Motor Action: Evidence for a Temporal Oscillator Controlling the Timing of Movement , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[23]  R J Bootsma,et al.  Visual information about time-to-collision between two objects. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  K. Bandeen-Roche,et al.  Appendix 1 , 2019, European Journal of Human Genetics.

[25]  Daniel Gopher,et al.  Workload: An examination of the concept. , 1986 .

[26]  D. Rosenbaum Perception and extrapolation of velocity and acceleration. , 1975 .

[27]  J. Gibson,et al.  Perception : essays in honor of James J. Gibson , 1974 .

[28]  W Schiff,et al.  Accuracy of judging time to arrival: effects of modality, trajectory, and gender. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Detection Theory: A User's Guide , 1991 .

[30]  B G Bardy,et al.  Visual Cues and Attention Demand in Locomotor Positioning , 1991, Perceptual and motor skills.

[31]  J T Todd,et al.  Visual information about moving objects. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  G J Andersen,et al.  Active control in interrupted dynamic spatial orientation: The detection of orientation change , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[33]  W Schiff,et al.  Information Used in Judging Impending Collision , 1979, Perception.

[34]  Alan Watt,et al.  Fundamentals of three-dimensional computer graphics , 1989 .

[35]  Jennifer Linda Blume Novak,et al.  Judgments of absolute time-to-contact in multiple object displays: evaluating the role of cognitive processes in arrival-time judgements , 1997 .

[36]  L. Brooks The Suppression of Visualization by Reading , 1967, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[37]  L. Mowafy,et al.  Mental extrapolation of perceptually driven spatial transformations , 1987 .

[38]  R. Rousseau,et al.  Time estimation and concurrent nontemporal processing: Specific interference from short-term-memory demands , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[39]  J. G. Hollands,et al.  Engineering Psychology and Human Performance , 1984 .