Time-locked Perceptual Fading Induced by Visual Transients

After prolonged fixation, a stationary object placed in the peripheral visual field fades and disappears from our visual awareness, especially at low luminance contrast (the Troxler effect). Here, we report that similar fading can be triggered by visual transients, such as additional visual stimuli flashed near the object, apparent motion, or a brief removal of the object itself (blinking). The fading occurs even without prolonged adaptation and is time-locked to the presentation of the visual transients. Experiments show that the effect of a flashed object decreased monotonically as a function of the distance from the target object. Consistent with this result, when apparent motion, consisting of a sequence of flashes was presented between stationary disks, these target disks perceptually disappeared as if erased by the moving object. Blinking the target disk, instead of flashing an additional visual object, turned out to be sufficient to induce the fading. The effect of blinking peaked around a blink duration of 80 msec. Our findings reveal a unique mechanism that controls the visibility of visual objects in a spatially selective and time-locked manner in response to transient visual inputs. Possible mechanisms underlying this phenomenon will be discussed.

[1]  J. Krauskopf Effect of retinal image stabilization on the appearance of heterochromatic targets. , 1963, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[2]  Lianggang Lou,et al.  Selective Peripheral Fading: Evidence for Inhibitory Sensory Effect of Attention , 1999, Perception.

[3]  R. L. Gregory,et al.  Perceptual filling in of artificially induced scotomas in human vision , 1991, Nature.

[4]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[5]  DH Hubel,et al.  Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[6]  Chromatic flicker. , 1969, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[7]  D. H. Kelly Motion and vision. II. Stabilized spatio-temporal threshold surface. , 1979, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[8]  M. A. Parasido Brightness perception and filling-in , 1991 .

[9]  G. Mowbray,et al.  Visual Masking , 1964, Nature.

[10]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Voluntary and Stimulus-Induced Attention Detected as Motion Sensation , 1993, Perception.

[11]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  The visual field representation in striate cortex of the macaque monkey: Asymmetries, anisotropies, and individual variability , 1984, Vision Research.

[12]  R. Desimone Visual attention mediated by biased competition in extrastriate visual cortex. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[13]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Perceptual filling-in: a parametric study , 1998, Vision Research.

[14]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[15]  F.J.J. Clarke,et al.  Visual Recovery Following Local Adaptation of the Peripheral Retina (Troxler's Effect) , 1961 .

[16]  J. Hell,et al.  Motion-induced blindness in normal observers , 2022 .

[17]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Enhanced Perception of Illusory Contours in the Lower Versus Upper Visual Hemifields , 1996, Science.