Social prioritisation in scene viewing and the effects of a spatial memory load
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Wendian Shi,et al. Increasing the load on executive working memory reduces the search performance in the natural scenes: Evidence from eye movements , 2021, Current Psychology.
[2] T. Foulsham,et al. Do cognitive load and ADHD traits affect the tendency to prioritise social information in scenes? , 2021, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[3] Deborah A. Cronin,et al. Visual and verbal working memory loads interfere with scene-viewing , 2019, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[4] M. Gamer,et al. Task instructions can accelerate the early preference for social features in naturalistic scenes , 2019, Royal Society Open Science.
[5] L. Downey,et al. A review of gaze entropy as a measure of visual scanning efficiency , 2019, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[6] E. Keogh,et al. Cognitive load selectively influences the interruptive effect of pain on attention , 2017, Pain.
[7] M. Gamer,et al. Top-down influence on gaze patterns in the presence of social features , 2017, PloS one.
[8] M. Gamer,et al. Orienting towards social features in naturalistic scenes is reflexive , 2017, PloS one.
[9] M. Gamer,et al. Preferential Processing of Social Features and Their Interplay with Physical Saliency in Complex Naturalistic Scenes , 2017, Front. Psychol..
[10] Mieke Donk,et al. Salient object changes influence overt attentional prioritization and object-based targeting in natural scenes , 2017, PloS one.
[11] Matthias Bethge,et al. Information-theoretic model comparison unifies saliency metrics , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[12] D. Pearson,et al. Oculomotor preparation as a rehearsal mechanism in spatial working memory , 2014, Cognition.
[13] J. King,et al. Working memory load and distraction: dissociable effects of visual maintenance and cognitive control , 2014, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[14] D. Pearson,et al. Oculomotor involvement in spatial working memory is task-specific , 2013, Cognition.
[15] Geraint Rees,et al. Visual Short-term Memory Load Reduces Retinotopic Cortex Response to Contrast , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[16] J. Theeuwes,et al. Overt is no better than covert when rehearsing visuo-spatial information in working memory , 2011, Memory & cognition.
[17] J. Theeuwes. Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. , 2010, Acta psychologica.
[18] Bryan R Burnham,et al. Cognitive load modulates attentional capture by color singletons during effortful visual search. , 2010, Acta psychologica.
[19] Pietro Perona,et al. Detection of objects in natural scenes with minimal or no attention , 2010 .
[20] A. Kingstone,et al. Saliency does not account for fixations to eyes within social scenes , 2009, Vision Research.
[21] R. VanRullen. Binding hardwired versus on-demand feature conjunctions , 2009 .
[22] T. Klingberg,et al. Common and unique components of inhibition and working memory: An fMRI, within-subjects investigation , 2008, Neuropsychologia.
[23] Wieske van Zoest,et al. Effects of Salience Are Short-Lived , 2008, Psychological science.
[24] A. Kingstone,et al. Social Attention and Real-World Scenes: The Roles of Action, Competition and Social Content , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[25] T. Foulsham,et al. What can saliency models predict about eye movements? Spatial and sequential aspects of fixations during encoding and recognition. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[26] A. Kingstone,et al. Gaze selection in complex social scenes , 2008 .
[27] T. Foulsham,et al. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: in press Visual saliency and semantic incongruency influence eye movements when , 2022 .
[28] Simon J. Thorpe,et al. Ultra-rapid object detection with saccadic eye movements: Visual processing speed revisited , 2006, Vision Research.
[29] B. Postle. Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain , 2006, Neuroscience.
[30] E. Viding,et al. Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[31] D. Pearson,et al. Oculomotor Control and the Maintenance of Spatially and Temporally Distributed Events in Visuo-Spatial Working Memory , 2003, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[32] N. Lavie,et al. The Role of Perceptual Load in Processing Distractor Faces , 2003, Psychological science.
[33] S. Pickering,et al. Development of Memory for Pattern and Path: Further Evidence for the Fractionation of Visuo-Spatial Memory , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[34] N. Lavie,et al. On the Efficiency of Visual Selective Attention: Efficient Visual Search Leads to Inefficient Distractor Rejection , 1997 .
[35] D. Pearson,et al. The Inner Eye and the Inner Scribe of Visuo-spatial Working Memory: Evidence from Developmental Fractionation , 1997 .
[36] Denis Fize,et al. Speed of processing in the human visual system , 1996, Nature.
[37] J. Myerson,et al. Selective interference with the maintenance of location information in working memory. , 1996 .
[38] N. Moray. Attention in Dichotic Listening: Affective Cues and the Influence of Instructions , 1959 .
[39] Susan M. Courtney,et al. Binding serial order to representations in working memory: a spatial/verbal dissociation , 2011, Memory & cognition.
[40] R. Desimone,et al. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.