Intact recognition, but attenuated adaptation, for biological motion in youth with autism spectrum disorder

Given the ecological importance of biological motion and its relevance to social cognition, considerable effort has been devoted over the past decade to studying biological motion perception in autism. However, previous studies have asked observers to detect or recognize briefly presented human actions placed in isolation, without spatial or temporal context. Research on typical populations has shown the influence of temporal context in biological motion perception: prolonged exposure to one action gives rise to an aftereffect that biases perception of a subsequently displayed action. Whether people with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) show such adaptation effects for biological motion stimuli remains unknown. To address this question, this study examined how well youth with ASD recognize ambiguous actions and adapt to recently‐observed actions. Compared to typically‐developing (TD) controls, youth with ASD showed no differences in perceptual boundaries between actions categories, indicating intact ability in recognizing actions. However, children with ASD showed weakened adaptation to biological motion. It is unlikely that the reduced action adaptability in autism was due to delayed developmental trajectory, as older children with ASD showed weaker adaptation to actions than younger children with ASD. Our results further suggest that high‐level (i.e., action) processing weakens with age for children with ASD, but this change may be accompanied by a potentially compensatory mechanism based on more involvement of low‐level (i.e., motion) processing. Autism Res 2016, 9: 1103–1113. © 2016 International Society for Autism Research, Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

[1]  K. Pelphrey,et al.  Brain Mechanisms for Social Perception , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[2]  M. Brosnan,et al.  Gestalt processing in autism: failure to process perceptual relationships and the implications for contextual understanding. , 2004, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[3]  Jeroen J. A. van Boxtel,et al.  Signature Movements Lead to Efficient Search for Threatening Actions , 2012, PloS one.

[4]  Mark W. Greenlee,et al.  Position-specific and position-invariant face aftereffects reflect the adaptation of different cortical areas , 2008, NeuroImage.

[5]  J. Wagemans,et al.  Global processing takes time: A meta-analysis on local-global visual processing in ASD. , 2013, Psychological bulletin.

[6]  Jeroen J. A. van Boxtel,et al.  Impaired Global, and Compensatory Local, Biological Motion Processing in People with High Levels of Autistic Traits , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[7]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews Face Identity Recognition in Autism Spectrum Disorders: a Review of Behavioral Studies , 2022 .

[8]  Naomi B. Pitskel,et al.  Neural signatures of autism , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[9]  Beatriz Luna,et al.  Missing the big picture: impaired development of global shape processing in autism , 2008, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[10]  E. Schopler,et al.  Toward objective classification of childhood autism: Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) , 1980, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[11]  M. Shiffrar,et al.  Comparison of visual sensitivity to human and object motion in autism spectrum disorder , 2010, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[12]  Ayse Pinar Saygin,et al.  Unaffected Perceptual Thresholds for Biological and Non-Biological Form-from-Motion Perception in Autism Spectrum Conditions , 2010, PloS one.

[13]  U. Frrrh Autism: explaining the enigma , 1989 .

[14]  Jennifer H. Pfeifer,et al.  Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  D. Burr,et al.  When the world becomes ‘too real’: a Bayesian explanation of autistic perception , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  Steven M. Thurman,et al.  Physical and Biological Constraints Govern Perceived Animacy of Scrambled Human Forms , 2013, Psychological science.

[17]  Joseph Piven,et al.  Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders is now a fully open access journal , 2012, Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders.

[18]  G. Rhodes,et al.  Atypical updating of face representations with experience in children with autism. , 2013, Developmental science.

[19]  Lawrie S. McKay,et al.  Vision in autism spectrum disorders , 2009, Vision Research.

[20]  Ruth Campbell,et al.  Atypical development of motion processing trajectories in autism , 2010 .

[21]  S. Bookheimer,et al.  Abnormal social reward processing in autism as indexed by pupillary responses to happy faces , 2012, Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders.

[22]  Janet Hui-wen Hsiao,et al.  NIMBLE: a kernel density model of saccade-based visual memory. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[23]  Michael S C Thomas,et al.  Development of motion processing in children with autism. , 2009, Developmental science.

[24]  J. Wagemans,et al.  Precise minds in uncertain worlds: predictive coding in autism. , 2014, Psychological review.

[25]  Nikolaus F. Troje,et al.  No evidence for impaired perception of biological motion in adults with autistic spectrum disorders , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  John A. Pyles,et al.  fMR-Adaptation Reveals Invariant Coding of Biological Motion on the Human STS , 2009, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[27]  Marcin Szczerbinski,et al.  Global and local perceptual style, field-independence, and central coherence: An attempt at concept validation. , 2009, Advances in cognitive psychology.

[28]  John O. Willis,et al.  Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence , 2014 .

[29]  A. Couteur,et al.  Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders , 1994, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[30]  Armin Scheurich,et al.  Gestalt Perception and Local-Global Processing in High-Functioning Autism , 2007, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[31]  S. Blakemore,et al.  Development of the adolescent brain: implications for executive function and social cognition. , 2006 .

[32]  G. Baird,et al.  No evidence for a fundamental visual motion processing deficit in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders , 2011, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[33]  Sarah-Jayne Blakemore,et al.  Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? , 2014, Annual review of psychology.

[34]  M. Pavlova Biological motion processing as a hallmark of social cognition. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[35]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  Global/Local Processing in Autism: Not a Disability, but a Disinclination , 2013, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[36]  Marco Iacoboni,et al.  Atypical neural networks for social orienting in autism spectrum disorders , 2011, NeuroImage.

[37]  G. Johansson Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .

[38]  Jay N Giedd,et al.  Anatomical brain magnetic resonance imaging of typically developing children and adolescents. , 2009, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[39]  J. Lange,et al.  A Model of Biological Motion Perception from Configural Form Cues , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  Hongjing Lu,et al.  Two forms of aftereffects induced by transparent motion reveal multilevel adaptation. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[41]  A. Klin,et al.  Two-year-olds with autism orient to nonsocial contingencies rather than biological motion , 2009, Nature.

[42]  N. Troje,et al.  The Inversion Effect in Biological Motion Perception: Evidence for a “Life Detector”? , 2006, Current Biology.

[43]  Simon Baron-Cohen,et al.  A test of central coherence theory: Can adults with high-functioning autism or Asperger syndrome integrate fragments of an object? , 2001, Cognitive neuropsychiatry.

[44]  Hongjing Lu,et al.  A biological motion toolbox for reading, displaying, and manipulating motion capture data in research settings. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[45]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  The reference frame of the motion aftereffect is retinotopic. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[46]  D. Burr,et al.  Atypicalities in Perceptual Adaptation in Autism Do Not Extend to Perceptual Causality , 2015, PloS one.

[47]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Adaptation aftereffects in the perception of gender from biological motion. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[48]  E. Bullmore,et al.  The role of MT+/V5 during biological motion perception in Asperger Syndrome: An fMRI study , 2007 .

[49]  Randolph Blake,et al.  Visual Recognition of Biological Motion is Impaired in Children With Autism , 2003, Psychological science.

[50]  Jeroen J. A. van Boxtel,et al.  A predictive coding perspective on autism spectrum disorders , 2013, Front. Psychology.

[51]  F. Simion,et al.  A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[52]  J. Movshon,et al.  Adaptation changes the direction tuning of macaque MT neurons , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[53]  HighWire Press The journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. , 1981 .

[54]  G. Rhodes,et al.  Reduced Face Aftereffects in Autism Are Not Due to Poor Attention , 2013, PloS one.

[55]  S. Bookheimer,et al.  Neural bases of gaze and emotion processing in children with autism spectrum disorders , 2011, Brain and behavior.

[56]  Peter Neri,et al.  Action Perception Is Intact in Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[57]  Jeroen J. A. van Boxtel,et al.  Retinotopic and non-retinotopic stimulus encoding in binocular rivalry and the involvement of feedback. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[58]  IQ Predicts Biological Motion Perception in Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2012, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[59]  Hongjing Lu,et al.  Joints and their relations as critical features in action discrimination: evidence from a classification image method. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[60]  David Whitney,et al.  The psychophysics of visual motion and global form processing in autism. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[61]  H. Bülthoff,et al.  Putting Actions in Context: Visual Action Adaptation Aftereffects Are Modulated by Social Contexts , 2014, PloS one.

[62]  Rp Hobson,et al.  Components of person perception: An investigation with autistic, non‐autistic retarded and typically developing children and adolescents , 1997 .

[63]  Martin A. Giese,et al.  Morphable Models for the Analysis and Synthesis of Complex Motion Patterns , 2000, International Journal of Computer Vision.

[64]  Andreia Santos,et al.  Recognition of biological motion in children with autistic spectrum disorders , 2008, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[65]  G. Rhodes,et al.  How is facial expression coded? , 2015, Journal of vision.

[66]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Recognizing Biological Motion and Emotions from Point-Light Displays in Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2012, PloS one.

[67]  Geoffrey Bird,et al.  Intact Facial Adaptation in Autistic Adults , 2014, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[68]  J. Frisby Seeing: Illusion, Brain and Mind , 1979 .

[69]  Antonino Casile,et al.  Critical features for the recognition of biological motion. , 2005, Journal of vision.

[70]  Hongjing Lu,et al.  Structural processing in biological motion perception. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[71]  D. Burr,et al.  Abnormal Adaptive Face-Coding Mechanisms in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2007, Current Biology.

[72]  U. Frith,et al.  Vagaries of Visual Perception in Autism , 2005, Neuron.

[73]  F. Happé Central coherence and theory of mind in autism: Reading homographs in context , 1997 .

[74]  B. Hubert,et al.  Enhanced Perceptual Functioning in Autism: An Update, and Eight Principles of Autistic Perception , 2006, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[75]  Maggie Shiffrar,et al.  The visual perception of motion by observers with autism spectrum disorders: A review and synthesis , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[76]  Alan L. F. Lee,et al.  A comparison of global motion perception using a multiple-aperture stimulus. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[77]  Nikolaus F. Troje,et al.  Perception of biological motion in autism spectrum disorders , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[78]  Kevin A. Pelphrey,et al.  Action representation in the superior temporal sulcus in children and adults: An fMRI study , 2012, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[79]  U. Frith,et al.  The Weak Coherence Account: Detail-focused Cognitive Style in Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2006, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[80]  J. Faubert,et al.  Motion Perception in Autism: A Complex Issue , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[81]  Kevin A. Pelphrey,et al.  Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience Disrupted Action Perception in Autism: Behavioral Evidence, Neuroendophenotypes, and Diagnostic Utility , 2022 .

[82]  Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Do Not Preferentially Attend to Biological Motion , 2012, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[83]  B. Leventhal,et al.  The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Generic: A Standard Measure of Social and Communication Deficits Associated with the Spectrum of Autism , 2000, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[84]  Frank E. Pollick,et al.  Do distinct atypical cortical networks process biological motion information in adults with Autism Spectrum Disorders? , 2012, NeuroImage.

[85]  B. Wicker,et al.  Brief Report: Recognition of Emotional and Non-emotional Biological Motion in Individuals with Autistic Spectrum Disorders , 2007, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.