Maximizing performance feedback effectiveness through videotape replay and a self-controlled learning environment.

This study was designed to examine whether participants who could control the schedule of performance feedback (KP) would learn differentially from those who received a rigid feedback schedule while learning a complex task. Participants (N = 48) were randomly assigned to self-controlled KP (SELF), summary KP (SUMMARY), yoked control (YOKE), or knowledge of results only (KR) conditions. Data collection consisted of an acquisition phase and a 4-day retention phase during which right-handed participants performed a left-handed ball throw. Overall, throwing form improved across trial blocks during acquisition, with the SUMMARY, SELF, and YOKE groups showing more improvement than the KR group. During retention, the SELF group retained a higher level of throwing form and accuracy in comparison to the other groups. Results suggest that when given the opportunity to control the feedback environment, learners require relatively less feedback to acquire skills and retain those skills at a level equivalent to or surpassing those who are given more feedback but receive it passively.

[1]  L. Hardy,et al.  Self-regulation training in sport and work. , 1988, Ergonomics.

[2]  Richard A. Magill,et al.  The Influence of Augmented Feedback on Skill Learning Depends on Characteristics of the Skill and the Learner , 1994 .

[3]  R. Schmidt,et al.  Knowledge of results and motor learning: a review and critical reappraisal. , 1984, Psychological bulletin.

[4]  J. A. Adams,et al.  Response feedback and motor learning. , 1972, Journal of experimental psychology.

[5]  B. Zimmerman Models of Self-Regulated Learning and Academic Achievement , 1989 .

[6]  Dennis Landin,et al.  The Role of Verbal Cues in Skill Learning , 1994 .

[7]  I. Bilodeau,et al.  Variable frequency of knowledge of results and the learning of a simple skill. , 1958, Journal of experimental psychology.

[8]  D. Watkins Student Perceptions of Factors Influencing Tertiary Learning , 1984 .

[9]  K. Newell Motor skill acquisition. , 1991, Annual review of psychology.

[10]  Wolfgang Schneider,et al.  Memory Development Between 2 and 20 , 1988 .

[11]  Edward L. Thorndike,et al.  The influence of delay in the aftereffect of a connection , 1935 .

[12]  J. R. Thomas,et al.  Effects of training on gender differences in overhand throwing: a brief quantitative literature analysis. , 1994, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[13]  Richard A. Schmidt,et al.  Optimizing summary knowledge of results for skill learning , 1990 .

[14]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  Children's thinking, 2nd ed. , 1991 .

[15]  G. R. Hancock,et al.  On the Problem of Two-Dimensional Error Scores: Measures and Analyses of Accuracy, Bias, and Consistency. , 1995, Journal of motor behavior.

[16]  Barbara L. McCombs,et al.  Self-Regulated Learning and Academic Achievement: A Phenomenological View , 1989 .

[17]  C. B. Walter,et al.  Kinematic and kinetic parameters as information feedback in motor skill acquisition , 1981 .

[18]  J J LAVERY,et al.  Retention of Simple Motor Skills as a Function of the Number of Trials by Which KR is Delayed , 1962, Perceptual and motor skills.

[19]  J. Sheahan Developing teaching skills. , 1981, Nursing focus.

[20]  K. Newell Knowledge Of Results And Motor Learning , 1976, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[21]  Judith E. Rink,et al.  Teaching Physical Education for Learning , 1985 .

[22]  J. R. Thomas,et al.  Gender differences across age in motor performance a meta-analysis. , 1985, Psychological bulletin.

[23]  Claude Alain,et al.  Should Common Optimal Movement Patterns Be Identified as the Criterion to Be Achieved? , 1996, Journal of motor behavior.

[24]  J. R. Thomas,et al.  Longitudinal change in throwing performance: gender differences. , 1991, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[25]  R N Singer,et al.  Subject-controlled performance feedback and learning of a closed motor skill. , 1995, Perceptual and motor skills.

[26]  J. Holt How Children Fail , 1964 .

[27]  Monica R. Wild The Behavior Pattern of Throwing and Some Observations Concerning its Course of Development in Children , 1938 .

[28]  Richard A. Magill,et al.  Motor Learning. Concepts and Applications , 1998 .

[29]  Linda S. Masser Critical Cues Help First-Grade Students’ Achievement in Handstands and Forward Rolls , 1993 .

[30]  R. Schmidt,et al.  Reduced frequency of knowledge of results enhances motor skill learning. , 1990 .

[31]  Richard A. Schmidt,et al.  The learning of generalized motor programs: Reducing the relative frequency of knowledge of results enhances memory. , 1989 .

[32]  William G. Anderson,et al.  What's Going on in Gym: Descriptive Studies of Physical Education Classes. Monograph 1. Motor Skills: Theory into Practice. , 1978 .

[33]  R. Schmidt,et al.  Augmented Kinematic Feedback for Motor Learning. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.

[34]  S. A. Wallace,et al.  Knowledge of performance and the learning of a closed motor skill. , 1979, Research quarterly.

[35]  Barry J. Zimmerman,et al.  Developing Self-Regulated Learners: Beyond Achievement to Self-Efficacy , 1996 .

[36]  R. Singer,et al.  Self-regulation and cognitive strategies in sport participation. , 1992 .

[37]  A Bandura,et al.  Representational guidance of action production in observational learning: a causal analysis. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.

[38]  Stephan P. Swinnen,et al.  Summary knowledge of results for skill acquisition: support for the guidance hypothesis , 1989 .

[39]  L. Carlton,et al.  Information feedback and the learning multiple-degree-of-freedom activities. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.

[40]  M. Weiss,et al.  Developmental modeling effects on the quantitative and qualitative aspects of motor performance. , 1990, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.