Neurocognition in depression: patients on and off medication versus healthy comparison subjects.

Patients with depression have neuropsychological deficits in attention, memory, psychomotor speed, processing speed, and executive function. It is not clear, however, whether neurocognition in depression is impaired in a global or nonspecific way or if specific cognitive domains are selectively impaired. This naturalistic cross-sectional study employed a computerized neurocognitive screening battery to evaluate 38 depressed, drug-free patients, compared to 31 patients who responded to antidepressant monotherapy and to 69 healthy comparison subjects. There was evidence for global neuropsychological impairment in untreated depressed patients. In patients who had been successfully treated, performance was improved but not normalized. There was also evidence for specific depression-related deficits in executive function and processing speed but not in memory, psychomotor speed, or reaction time. Although depressed patients have global neurocognitive impairments, deficits in certain cognitive domains are more important than in others. In particular, impairments are noted in tests of executive control and in tests that demand effortful attention. Information processing speed is also impaired but not reaction time. Computerized testing in the clinic setting demonstrates a range of neurocognitive problems in patients with depression. These problems may have a bearing on treatment and outcome.

[1]  H. Kraemer,et al.  Use of placebo control groups in evaluating efficacy of treatment of unipolar major depression , 2000, Biological Psychiatry.

[2]  Differentiation of cognitive and motor slowing in the Digit Symbol Test (DST): differences between depression and schizophrenia , 1998 .

[3]  K. Oedegaard,et al.  Impairment across executive functions in recurrent major depression , 2004, Nordic journal of psychiatry.

[4]  P. Fossati,et al.  Influence of age and executive functioning on verbal memory of inpatients with depression. , 2002, Journal of affective disorders.

[5]  H E ROSVOLD,et al.  A continuous performance test of brain damage. , 1956, Journal of consulting psychology.

[6]  E C Hills,et al.  Effect of stimulus number, target-to-distractor ratio, and motor speed on visual spatial search quality following traumatic brain injury. , 1997, Brain injury.

[7]  Jane S. Paulsen,et al.  Clinical and neuropsychological comparison of psychotic depression with nonpsychotic depression and schizophrenia. , 1996, The American journal of psychiatry.

[8]  Til Wykes,et al.  Effects of depressed mood on objective and subjective measures of attention. , 2003, The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[9]  K. Maurer,et al.  Bias in computerized neuropsychological assessment of depressive disorders caused by computer attitude , 2002, Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica.

[10]  C Stough,et al.  Evidence of an early information processing speed deficit in unipolar major depression , 2002, Psychological Medicine.

[11]  Richard J. Porter,et al.  Neurocognitive impairment in drug-free patients with major depressive disorder , 2003, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[12]  R. Kane,et al.  Computerized assessment in neuropsychology: A review of tests and test batteries , 1992, Neuropsychology Review.

[13]  L. Parsons,et al.  Reciprocal limbic-cortical function and negative mood: converging PET findings in depression and normal sadness. , 1999, The American journal of psychiatry.

[14]  A. Moffoot,et al.  An analysis of memory dysfunction in major depression. , 1995, Journal of affective disorders.

[15]  D. Schopflocher,et al.  Neuropsychological test performance in depressed patients before and after drug therapy. , 1984, Biological psychiatry.

[16]  A. Schatzberg,et al.  Cortisol activity and cognitive changes in psychotic major depression. , 2001, The American journal of psychiatry.

[17]  W. McCall,et al.  Cognitive deficits are associated with functional impairment in severely depressed patients , 2003, Psychiatry Research.

[18]  Markus Kiefer,et al.  Executive control deficit in depression: event-related potentials in a Go/Nogo task , 2003, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.

[19]  G. Niederehe,et al.  Depression and memory impairment: a meta-analysis of the association, its pattern, and specificity. , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[20]  J. Jolles,et al.  Cognitive functioning in young and middle-aged unmedicated out-patients with major depression: testing the effort and cognitive speed hypotheses , 2003, Psychological Medicine.

[21]  M. Caligiuri,et al.  Motor and cognitive aspects of motor retardation in depression. , 2000, Journal of affective disorders.

[22]  N. Landrø,et al.  Neuropsychological function in nonpsychotic unipolar major depression. , 2001, Neuropsychiatry, neuropsychology, and behavioral neurology.

[23]  K. Hugdahl,et al.  Attention profile in schizophrenia compared with depression: differential effects of processing speed, selective attention and vigilance , 2003, Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica.

[24]  A. N. Brand,et al.  Recall and recognition memory deficits in depression. , 1992, Journal of affective disorders.

[25]  W Hulstijn,et al.  Retardation in depression: assessment by means of simple motor tasks. , 1999, Journal of affective disorders.

[26]  N. Raoux,et al.  Executive functioning and verbal memory in young patients with unipolar depression and schizophrenia , 1999, Psychiatry Research.

[27]  M. Rinck,et al.  Selective memory and memory deficits in depressed inpatients , 2003, Depression and anxiety.

[28]  R. Gur,et al.  Computerized Neurocognitive Scanning: I. Methodology and Validation in Healthy People , 2001, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[29]  D. Barch,et al.  Working memory and prefrontal cortex dysfunction: specificity to schizophrenia compared with major depression , 2003, Biological Psychiatry.

[30]  M. Åsberg,et al.  Memory effects of clomipramine treatment: Relationship to CSF monoamine metabolites and drug concentrations in plasma , 1991, Biological Psychiatry.

[31]  Robert H. Paul,et al.  Impairments of attention and effort among patients with major affective disorders. , 2001, The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[32]  R. Dolan,et al.  Cognitive function in depression: its relationship to the presence and severity of intellectual decline , 1994, Psychological Medicine.

[33]  P. Rubé,et al.  L’examen Clinique en Psychologie , 1959 .

[34]  P. Appelbaum,et al.  Neuroleptics: effects on neuropsychological function in chronic schizophrenic patients. , 1990, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[35]  P. Fossati,et al.  Qualitative analysis of verbal fluency in depression , 2003, Psychiatry Research.

[36]  P. Schlattmann,et al.  Time-related cognitive deficiency in four different types of depression , 2001, Psychiatry Research.

[37]  H. Demaree,et al.  The Neuropsychology of Depression: A Literature Review and Preliminary Model , 2003, Neuropsychology Review.

[38]  F. Zitman,et al.  Fine motor retardation and depression. , 1996, Journal of psychiatric research.

[39]  M. Jarvik,et al.  Memory Functions in Depression: Improvement With Antidepressant Medication , 1976 .

[40]  B. Pollock,et al.  Persistence of cognitive impairment in geriatric patients following antidepressant treatment: a randomized, double-blind clinical trial with nortriptyline and paroxetine. , 2003, Journal of psychiatric research.

[41]  P. Fossati,et al.  [Executive functioning in unipolar depression: a review]. , 2002, L'Encephale.

[42]  G. MacQueen,et al.  Recollection memory deficits in patients with major depressive disorder predicted by past depressions but not current mood state or treatment status , 2002, Psychological Medicine.

[43]  K. Krishnan,et al.  Does antidepressant therapy improve cognition in elderly depressed patients? , 2003, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[44]  B. Ravnkilde,et al.  Cognitive deficits in major depression. , 2002, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[45]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .