Human memory retrieval as Lévy foraging

When people attempt to recall as many words as possible from a specific category (e.g., animal names) their retrievals occur sporadically over an extended temporal period. Retrievals decline as recall progresses, but short retrieval bursts can occur even after tens of minutes of performing the task. To date, efforts to gain insight into the nature of retrieval from this fundamental phenomenon of semantic memory have focused primarily upon the exponential growth rate of cumulative recall. Here we focus upon the time intervals between retrievals. We expected and found that, for each participant in our experiment, these intervals conformed to a Levy distribution suggesting that the Levy flight dynamics that characterize foraging behavior may also characterize retrieval from semantic memory. The closer the exponent on the inverse square power-law distribution of retrieval intervals approximated the optimal foraging value of 2, the more efficient was the retrieval. At an abstract dynamical level, foraging for particular foods in one's niche and searching for particular words in one's memory must be similar processes if particular foods and particular words are randomly and sparsely located in their respective spaces at sites that are not known a priori. We discuss whether Levy dynamics imply that memory processes, like foraging, are optimized in an ecological way.

[1]  H. Larralde,et al.  Lévy walk patterns in the foraging movements of spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) , 2003, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[2]  George L. Hunt,et al.  Foraging in a fractal environment: Spatial patterns in a marine predator-prey system , 1992, Landscape Ecology.

[3]  J. Klafter,et al.  Anomalous diffusion spreads its wings , 2005 .

[4]  M. Levandowsky,et al.  Chemosensory Responses of Acanthamoeba castellanii: Visual Analysis of Random Movement and Responses to Chemical Signals , 1996, The Journal of eukaryotic microbiology.

[5]  B. Cole Fractal time in animal behaviour: the movement activity of Drosophila , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[6]  M. E. J. Newman,et al.  Power laws, Pareto distributions and Zipf's law , 2005 .

[7]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Reflections of the Environment in Memory Form of the Memory Functions , 2022 .

[8]  D. Macdonald,et al.  Scale‐free dynamics in the movement patterns of jackals , 2002 .

[9]  H. Stanley,et al.  Optimizing the success of random searches , 1999, Nature.

[10]  M. Levandowsky,et al.  Swimming behavior and chemosensory responses in the protistan microzooplankton as a function of the hydrodynamic regime , 1988 .

[11]  D. Sims,et al.  Minimizing errors in identifying Lévy flight behaviour of organisms. , 2007, The Journal of animal ecology.

[12]  P. A. Prince,et al.  Lévy flight search patterns of wandering albatrosses , 1996, Nature.

[13]  W. A. Bousfield,et al.  An Analysis of Sequences of Restricted Associative Responses , 1944 .

[14]  H. Stanley,et al.  Lévy flights in random searches , 2000 .

[15]  D'arcy W. Thompson On growth and form i , 1943 .

[16]  F. Halzen,et al.  What are high-energy cosmic rays? , 1995 .

[17]  Frederic Bartumeus,et al.  ANIMAL SEARCH STRATEGIES: A QUANTITATIVE RANDOM‐WALK ANALYSIS , 2005 .

[18]  J. Wixted,et al.  Analyzing the dynamics of free recall: An integrative review of the empirical literature , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[19]  Sergey V. Buldyrev,et al.  Lévy flights search patterns of biological organisms , 2001 .

[20]  Salvador Pueyo,et al.  Diversity : between neutrality and structure , 2006 .