The relation between television exposure and theory of mind among preschoolers

This study explored the relation between preschoolers' television exposure and one important indicator of cognitive processing called theory of mind (ToM). A total of 107 preschoolers and their parents provided data on the preschoolers' television exposure (including both intentional viewing and exposure via background television), parent–child discussion of television, and preschoolers' ToM. The results indicated that preschoolers who were exposed to more background television and who had a television in their bedroom performed more poorly on ToM assessments compared with other children. Parent–child discussion of television was positively related to ToM performance, however. These results have implications for how we understand the effects of television on preschoolers.

[1]  R. Banerjee,et al.  Longitudinal effects of theory of mind on later peer relations: the role of prosocial behavior. , 2012, Developmental psychology.

[2]  W. Collins,et al.  Effects of adult commentary on children's comprehension and inferences about a televised aggressive portrayal. , 1981, Child development.

[3]  J. Astington,et al.  A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[4]  Haejung Paik,et al.  The Effects of Television Violence on Antisocial Behavior: A Meta-Analysis1 , 1994 .

[5]  Shalom M. Fisch,et al.  The impact of Sesame Street on preschool children: A review and synthesis of 30 years' research. , 1999 .

[6]  Charles Fernyhough,et al.  Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. , 2002, Child development.

[7]  H. Wellman,et al.  Young children's storybooks as a source of mental state information , 2000 .

[8]  L. Villanueva,et al.  Mothers' use of cognitive state verbs in picture-book reading and the development of children's understanding of mind: a longitudinal study. , 2007, Child development.

[9]  L. Moses,et al.  Demographics, Parenting, and Theory of Mind in Preschool Children , 2003 .

[10]  Joanne Cantor,et al.  Reducing the Aggression-Promoting Effect of Violent Cartoons By Increasing Children's Fictional Involvement with the Victim: A Study of Active Mediation , 2000 .

[11]  Amy I. Nathanson,et al.  Predictors of children's interest in violent television programs , 1997 .

[12]  Kang Lee,et al.  Effects of a Punitive Environment on Children's Executive Functioning: A Natural Experiment , 2011 .

[13]  Barbara J. Wilson Media and Children's Aggression, Fear, and Altruism , 2008, The Future of children.

[14]  S. Gortmaker,et al.  Televisions in the Bedrooms of Racial/Ethnic Minority Children: How Did They Get There and How Do We Get Them Out? , 2009, Clinical pediatrics.

[15]  J. Smetana,et al.  Reciprocal associations between young children's developing moral judgments and theory of mind. , 2012, Developmental psychology.

[16]  N. Fox,et al.  The relations among theory of mind, behavioral inhibition, and peer interactions in early childhood. , 2012, Social development.

[17]  Jessica Taylor Piotrowski,et al.  Background Television in the Homes of US Children , 2012, Pediatrics.

[18]  Baris Korkmaz,et al.  Theory of Mind and Neurodevelopmental Disorders of Childhood , 2011, Pediatric Research.

[19]  J. Astington,et al.  Language and theory of mind: meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. , 2007, Child development.

[20]  G. Boiarsky,et al.  Formal features in children's science television: Sound effects, visual pace, and topic shifts , 1999 .

[21]  M. Maybery,et al.  Specific language impairment, theory of mind, and visual perspective taking: evidence for simulation theory and the developmental role of language. , 2006, Child development.

[22]  J. Singer,et al.  Preschoolers' attributions of mental states in pretense. , 1997, Child development.

[23]  L. Moses,et al.  Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. , 2001, Child development.

[24]  Patti M. Valkenburg,et al.  Developmental Changes in Infants’ and Toddlers’ Attention to Television Entertainment , 2004, Commun. Res..

[25]  A. Bandura Social Cognitive Theory of Mass Communication , 2001 .

[26]  L. R. Huesmann,et al.  Psychological Processes Promoting the Relation Between Exposure to Media Violence and Aggressive Behavior by the Viewer , 1986 .

[27]  Daniel R. Anderson,et al.  The effects of background television on the toy play behavior of very young children. , 2008, Child development.

[28]  John H. Flavell,et al.  Do young children think of television images as pictures or real objects , 1990 .

[29]  Marjorie Taylor,et al.  The relation between individual differences in fantasy and theory of mind. , 1997, Child development.

[30]  S. Sisson,et al.  How did the television get in the child's bedroom? Analysis of family interviews. , 2012, Preventive medicine.

[31]  H. Wellman,et al.  Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. , 2004, Child development.

[32]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Children's understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance-reality distinction. , 1988, Child development.

[33]  J. Dunn,et al.  Understanding Others, and Individual Differences in Friendship Interactions in Young Children , 2001 .

[34]  Catherine A. Taylor,et al.  Television exposure as a risk factor for aggressive behavior among 3-year-old children. , 2009, Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine.

[35]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  What can The Study of Cognitive Development Reveal about Children's Ability to Appreciate and Cope with Advertising? , 2005 .

[36]  Shalom M. Fisch,et al.  "G" Is for Growing: Thirty Years of Research on Children and "Sesame Street.". , 2001 .

[37]  J. Dunn,et al.  Theory of mind, emotion understanding, language, and family background: individual differences and interrelations. , 1999, Child development.

[38]  R. Kahn,et al.  Prevalence, correlates, and trajectory of television viewing among infants and toddlers. , 2002, Pediatrics.

[39]  Lauren A Murphy,et al.  The impact of background television on parent-child interaction. , 2009, Child development.

[40]  Daniel R. Anderson,et al.  Form and content: looking at visual features of television. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[41]  Chris Moore,et al.  Exposure to media and theory-of-mind development in preschoolers , 2010 .

[42]  Mabel L. Rice,et al.  Words from Sesame Street : learning vocabulary while viewing , 1990 .

[43]  C. Hughes,et al.  Nature, Nurture, and Individual Differences in Early Understanding of Mind , 1999 .

[44]  M. Callanan,et al.  Metarepresentation in action: 3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds' developing theories of mind in parent-child conversations. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[45]  J. Dunn,et al.  ‘You can cry, mum’: The social and developmental implications of talk about internal states , 1991 .

[46]  Josef Perner,et al.  Theory of mind is contagious - you catch it from your sibs , 1994 .

[47]  R. Baillargeon,et al.  Do 15-Month-Old Infants Understand False Beliefs? , 2005, Science.

[48]  V. Slaughter,et al.  Theory of mind and peer acceptance in preschool children , 2002 .

[49]  V. Rideout,et al.  Digital Childhood: Electronic Media and Technology Use Among Infants, Toddlers, and Preschoolers , 2007, Pediatrics.

[50]  L. Pagani,et al.  Early Exposure to Media Violence and Later Child Adjustment , 2012, Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics : JDBP.

[51]  Marc de Rosnay,et al.  Conversation and theory of mind: Do children talk their way to socio-cognitive understanding? , 2006 .

[52]  Sandra L. Calvert,et al.  Formal production features of infant and toddler DVDs. , 2009, Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine.

[53]  W. Andrew Collins,et al.  Age-related aspects of comprehension and inference from a televised dramatic narrative , 1978 .