Localizing the sources of two independent noises: role of time varying amplitude differences.

Listeners localized the free-field sources of either one or two simultaneous and independently generated noise bursts. Listeners' localization performance was better when localizing one rather than two sound sources. With two sound sources, localization performance was better when the listener was provided prior information about the location of one of them. Listeners also localized two simultaneous noise bursts that had sinusoidal amplitude modulation (AM) applied, in which the modulation envelope was in-phase across the two source locations or was 180° out-of-phase. The AM was employed to investigate a hypothesis as to what process listeners might use to localize multiple sound sources. The results supported the hypothesis that localization of two sound sources might be based on temporal-spectral regions of the combined waveform in which the sound from one source was more intense than that from the other source. The interaural information extracted from such temporal-spectral regions might provide reliable estimates of the sound source location that produced the more intense sound in that temporal-spectral region.

[1]  B. Bauer,et al.  Phasor analysis of some stereophonic phenomena , 1962 .

[2]  G. Eberle,et al.  Localization of amplitude-modulated high-frequency noise. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[3]  Barbara G Shinn-Cunningham,et al.  Influences of modulation and spatial separation on detection of a masked broadband target. , 2008, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[4]  Klaus Hartung,et al.  Localization in the Presence of a Distracter and Reverberation in the Frontal Horizontal Plane. I. Psychoacoustical Data , 2002 .

[5]  F L Wightman,et al.  Detectability of a pulsed tone in the presence of a masker with time-varying interaural correlation. , 1978, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[6]  Adrian K C Lee,et al.  Localization interference between components in an auditory scene. , 2009, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[7]  D. Grantham,et al.  Binaural interference in the free field. , 2010, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[8]  Virginia Best,et al.  Separation of concurrent broadband sound sources by human listeners. , 2004, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[9]  H S Colburn,et al.  The precedence effect. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[10]  R. Gilkey,et al.  Sound localization in noise: the effect of signal-to-noise ratio. , 1996, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[11]  H S Colburn,et al.  Speech intelligibility and localization in a multi-source environment. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[12]  E. Langendijk,et al.  Sound localization in the presence of one or two distracters. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[13]  W. Hartmann,et al.  The acoustical bright spot and mislocalization of tones by human listeners. , 2010, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[14]  C. Faller,et al.  Source localization in complex listening situations: selection of binaural cues based on interaural coherence. , 2004, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[15]  G. F. Kuhn Physical acoustics and measurements pertaining to directional hearing , 1983 .

[16]  M. Gardner Image fusion, broadening, and displacement in sound location. , 1969, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[17]  DeLiang Wang,et al.  Sequential Organization of Speech in Reverberant Environments by Integrating Monaural Grouping and Binaural Localization , 2010, IEEE Transactions on Audio, Speech, and Language Processing.

[18]  R L Freyman,et al.  The role of perceived spatial separation in the unmasking of speech. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[19]  T E Hanna,et al.  Discrimination of reproducible noise as a function of bandwidth and duration , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[20]  Ville Pulkki,et al.  Directional perception of distributed sound sources. , 2011, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[21]  Hamish Meffin,et al.  Selective filtering to spurious localization cues in the mammalian auditory brainstem. , 2009, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[22]  F L Wightman,et al.  Detectability of varying interaural temporal differences. , 1978, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[23]  Terry T. Takahashi,et al.  Localization and identification of concurrent sounds in the owl’s auditory space map. , 2009 .

[24]  F L Wightman,et al.  Headphone simulation of free-field listening. II: Psychophysical validation. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[25]  Virginia Best,et al.  Sound localization with a preceding distractor. , 2007, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[26]  D. Grantham,et al.  Horizontal-Plane Localization of Noise and Speech Signals by Postlingually Deafened Adults Fitted With Bilateral Cochlear Implants* , 2007, Ear and hearing.

[27]  B C Wheeler,et al.  Localization of multiple sound sources with two microphones. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[28]  Frederic L. Wightman,et al.  Detectability of varying interaural temporal differencesa) , 1978 .

[29]  Volker Hohmann,et al.  Auditory model based direction estimation of concurrent speakers from binaural signals , 2011, Speech Commun..

[30]  W. Hartmann,et al.  Localization of sound in rooms, III: Onset and duration effects. , 1986, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[31]  Leslie R Bernstein,et al.  Enhancing interaural-delay-based extents of laterality at high frequencies by using "transposed stimuli". , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.