Clinical Validation and Cognitive Elaboration: Signs That Encourage Sustained Recycling

Three field experiments coupled the clinical psychology concept of validation with Elaboration Likelihood Model-Heuristic-Systematic Model theorizing to increase the influence of persuasive messages on aluminum can recycling. Signs that validated students' complaints that aluminum can recycling was inconvenient, but persuaded them to recycle anyhow (validate-persuade) were expected to reduce reactance, increase scrutiny and cognitive elaboration, and result in longer term behavior change. Across these 3 experiments, signs influenced recycling relative to baseline; a persuasive message was more influential than convenience; and clinical validation received support as a way to increase message scrutiny, cognitive elaboration, and sustained behavior change.

[1]  Robert L. Mason,et al.  Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 2003 .

[2]  Carol M. Werner,et al.  Changing Homeowners’ Behaviors Involving Toxic Household Chemicals: A Psychological, Multilevel Approach , 2001 .

[3]  T. Jahn,et al.  Sustainability and the social sciences : a cross-disciplinary approach to integrating environmental considerations into theoretical reorientation , 1999 .

[4]  Carol M. Werner,et al.  Designing Effective Instructional Signs With Schema Theory , 1998 .

[5]  P. Stern,et al.  Influences on Attitude-Behavior Relationships , 1995 .

[6]  R. Petty,et al.  Elaboration as a Determinant of Attitude Strength: Creating Attitudes That Are Persistent, Resistant, and Predictive of Behavior , 1995 .

[7]  Russell H. Fazio,et al.  Attitudes as object-evaluation associations: Determinants, consequences, and correlates of attitude accessibility. , 1995 .

[8]  Paul H. White,et al.  Race of source effects in the elaboration likelihood model. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[9]  C. Carver,et al.  Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales , 1994 .

[10]  Jeffrey Blodgett,et al.  The effects of perceived justice on complainants' negative word-of-mouth behavior and repatronage intentions , 1993 .

[11]  R. D. Young,et al.  Changing Behavior and Making it Stick The Conceptualization and Management of Conservation Behavior , 1993 .

[12]  S. Chaiken,et al.  The psychology of attitudes. , 1993 .

[13]  R. Cialdini Influence: Science and practice, 3rd ed. , 1993 .

[14]  R. Cialdini,et al.  When tactical pronouncements of change become real change : the case of reciprocal persuasion , 1992 .

[15]  J. Freedman,et al.  Inferred values and the reverse-incentive effect in induced compliance. , 1992 .

[16]  Blair T. Johnson,et al.  Involvement and Persuasion: Types, Traditions, and the Evidence , 1990 .

[17]  John T. Cacioppo,et al.  Involvement and Persuasion: Tradition Versus Integration , 1990 .

[18]  R. Fazio Multiple Processes by which Attitudes Guide Behavior: The Mode Model as an Integrative Framework , 1990 .

[19]  Blair T. Johnson,et al.  Effects of involvement on persuasion: a meta-analysis , 1989 .

[20]  A Agresti,et al.  Tutorial on modeling ordered categorical response data. , 1989, Psychological bulletin.

[21]  Franziska Marquart,et al.  Communication and persuasion : central and peripheral routes to attitude change , 1988 .

[22]  S. Chaiken The heuristic model of persuasion. , 1987 .

[23]  Stuart Oskamp,et al.  Increasing Community Recycling with Persuasive Communication and Public Commitment , 1986 .

[24]  E. Redman,et al.  Postpartum Depression: An Interactional View. , 1986 .

[25]  John T. Cacioppo,et al.  The Elaboration Likelihood Model of Persuasion , 1986, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology.

[26]  J. Coyne,et al.  TOWARD A THEORY OF FRAMES AND REFRAMING: THE SOCIAL NATURE OF FRAMES* , 1985 .

[27]  R. Cialdini Influence: Science and Practice , 1984 .

[28]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  The Effects of Involvement on Responses to Argument Quantity and Quality: Central and Peripheral Routes to Persuasion , 1984 .

[29]  R. D. Robertson Preserving the Environment: New Strategies for Behavior Change. By E. Scott Geller, Richard A. Winett, Peter B. Everett , 1982 .

[30]  J. F. Alexander,et al.  Understanding the principles of family functioning. , 1982 .

[31]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Personal involvement as a determinant of argument based persuasion , 1981 .

[32]  J. Brehm,et al.  Psychological Reactance: A Theory of Freedom and Control , 1981 .

[33]  M. Heesacker,et al.  Effects of rhetorical questions on persuasion: A cognitive response analysis. , 1981 .

[34]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Attitudes and Persuasion: Classic and Contemporary Approaches , 1981 .

[35]  E. Hatfield,et al.  Chronic sorrow revisited: parent vs. professional depiction of the adjustment of parents of mentally retarded children. , 1981, The American journal of orthopsychiatry.

[36]  J. Gottman Marital Interaction: Experimental Investigations , 1980 .

[37]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Issue involvement can increase or decrease persuasion by enhancing message-relevant cognitive responses. , 1979 .

[38]  John T. Cacioppo,et al.  Effects of Forwarning of Persuasive Intent and Involvement on Cognitive Responses and Persuasion , 1979 .

[39]  E. Langer,et al.  The Mindlessness of Ostensibly Thoughtful Action: The Role of "Placebic" Information in Interpersonal Interaction , 1978 .

[40]  B. Kintz,et al.  Computational handbook of statistics, 2nd ed. , 1977 .

[41]  Robert S. Baron,et al.  Distraction Can Enhance or Reduce Yielding to Propaganda: Thought Disruption Versus Effort Justification , 1976 .

[42]  B. Kintz,et al.  Computational Handbook of Statistics , 1968 .

[43]  A. Greenwald 6 – Cognitive Learning, Cognitive Response to Persuasion, and Attitude Change1 , 1968 .

[44]  Timothy C. Brock,et al.  Communication discrepancy and intent to persuade as determinants of counterargument production , 1967 .

[45]  C. Rogers,et al.  Client-Centered Therapy , 1951 .

[46]  M. Sherif,et al.  The psychology of attitudes. , 1946, Psychological review.