Pathological gamblers discount probabilistic rewards less steeply than matched controls.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] A.,et al. Self-control in the monkey Macacafascicularis , 2010 .
[2] W. Bickel,et al. Delay discounting and substance abuse-dependence. , 2010 .
[3] J. Anker,et al. Delay discounting as a predictor of drug abuse. , 2010 .
[4] G. Madden,et al. Impulsivity: The Behavioral and Neurological Science of Discounting , 2010 .
[5] G. Madden,et al. Discounting and Pathological Gambling , 2010 .
[6] Christopher E. Bullock,et al. Reinforcer accumulation in a token-reinforcement context with pigeons. , 2008, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[7] D. Hodgins,et al. Probability discounting among gamblers: Differences across problem gambling severity and affect-regulation expectancies , 2008 .
[8] K C Winters,et al. DSM-IV pathological gambling in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication , 2008, Psychological Medicine.
[9] Elizabeth A. Olson,et al. Adolescents' performance on delay and probability discounting tasks: contributions of age, intelligence, executive functioning, and self-reported externalizing behavior. , 2007, Personality and individual differences.
[10] W. Bickel,et al. Probability discounting among cigarette smokers and nonsmokers: molecular analysis discerns group differences , 2007, Behavioural pharmacology.
[11] W. Bickel,et al. Behavioral and neuroeconomics of drug addiction: competing neural systems and temporal discounting processes. , 2007, Drug and alcohol dependence.
[12] G. Badger,et al. Delay discounting predicts postpartum relapse to cigarette smoking among pregnant women. , 2007, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.
[13] Carla H. Lagorio,et al. Toward an Animal Model of Gambling: Delay Discounting and the Allure of Unpredictable Outcomes , 2007, Journal of Gambling Studies.
[14] Jesse Dallery,et al. Delay discounting predicts cigarette smoking in a laboratory model of abstinence reinforcement , 2007, Psychopharmacology.
[15] J. MacKillop,et al. Divergent Validity of Measures of Cognitive Distortions, Impulsivity, and Time Perspective in Pathological Gambling , 2006, Journal of Gambling Studies.
[16] B. Grant,et al. Comorbidity of DSM-IV pathological gambling and other psychiatric disorders: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. , 2005, The Journal of clinical psychiatry.
[17] W. Woolverton,et al. Effects of clomipramine on self-control choice in Lewis and Fischer 344 rats , 2005, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.
[18] R. McCormick,et al. Follow-up of male pathological gamblers after treatment: The relationship of intellectual variables to relapse , 1991, Journal of Gambling Studies.
[19] Gregory J. Madden,et al. Impulsivity (delay discounting) as a predictor of acquisition of IV cocaine self-administration in female rats , 2005, Psychopharmacology.
[20] L. Green,et al. A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.
[21] Kris N Kirby,et al. Heroin and cocaine abusers have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than alcoholics or non-drug-using controls. , 2004, Addiction.
[22] M. Dixon,et al. Delay discounting by pathological gamblers. , 2003, Journal of applied behavior analysis.
[23] S. M. Alessi,et al. Pathological gambling severity is associated with impulsivity in a delay discounting procedure , 2003, Behavioural Processes.
[24] Daniel D. Holt,et al. Is discounting impulsive? Evidence from temporal and probability discounting in gambling and non-gambling college students , 2003, Behavioural Processes.
[25] Daniel D. Holt,et al. Discounting delayed and probabilistic rewards: Processes and traits , 2003 .
[26] N. Petry. Pathological gamblers, with and without substance use disorders, discount delayed rewards at high rates. , 2001, Journal of abnormal psychology.
[27] W. Bickel,et al. Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug dependence: delay discounting processes. , 2001, Addiction.
[28] N. Petry. Gambling problems in substance abusers are associated with increased sexual risk behaviors. , 2000, Addiction.
[29] S. Mitchell,et al. Measures of impulsivity in cigarette smokers and non-smokers , 1999, Psychopharmacology.
[30] N. Petry,et al. Excessive discounting of delayed rewards in substance abusers with gambling problems. , 1999, Drug and alcohol dependence.
[31] W. Bickel,et al. Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[32] L Green,et al. Amount of reward has opposite effects on the discounting of delayed and probabilistic outcomes. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[33] J. May,et al. Self-control in the monkeymacaca fascicularis , 1996 .
[34] K. Kirby,et al. Modeling Myopic Decisions: Evidence for Hyperbolic Delay-Discounting within Subjects and Amounts , 1995 .
[35] Linda C. Sobell,et al. Timeline Follow-Back A Technique for Assessing Self-Reported Alcohol Consumption , 1992 .
[36] D. Cross,et al. Subjective probability and delay. , 1991, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[37] H. Rachlin. Why do People Gamble and Keep Gambling despite Heavy Losses? , 1990 .
[38] M. Commons. The effect of delay and of intervening events on reinforcement value , 2013 .
[39] H. Lesieur,et al. The South Oaks Gambling Screen (SaGS): A New Instrument for the Identification of Pathological Gamblers , 2010 .
[40] J. E. Mazur. An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. , 1987 .
[41] F. J. Evans,et al. New Data from the Addiction Severity Index Reliability and Validity in Three Centers , 1985, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.
[42] H. Eysenck,et al. Impulsiveness and Venturesomeness: Their Position in a Dimensional System of Personality Description , 1978, Psychological reports.