Quantifying color appearance visually and instrumentally

Color appearance can be measured visually or instrumentally. Visually, the color sample of interest is compared to similar samples from an atlas associated with an equally visually spaced color order system. The illumination, and thus chromatic adaptation, must be controlled, and simultaneous-contrast effects must be avoided during the comparison. The task is thus quite different from the everyday evaluation of colors in a complex scene. If instrumental measurement is used, one of the recently developed models of chromatic adaptation must be combined with an appropriate model of color vision and an analytical description of an adequate uniform color space to provide measures of color appearance. Progress in solving these difficult problems, mostly documented in previous articles in this journal, is described in the present review article.

[1]  Oscar Estévez Uscanga,et al.  On the fundamental data-base of normal and dichromatic color vision , 1979 .

[2]  R. Hunt,et al.  The perception of color in 1 degree fields for different states of adaptation. , 1953, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[3]  Dorothy Nickerson,et al.  OSA uniform color scale samples: A unique set , 1981 .

[4]  R. Hunt Light and dark adaptation and the perception of color. , 1952, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[5]  On the geometry of the OSA uniform color scales committee space , 1981 .

[6]  R. Hunt,et al.  A model of colour vision for predicting colour appearance , 1982 .

[7]  Anders Hård,et al.  NCS—Natural Color System: A Swedish Standard for Color Notation , 1981 .

[8]  Yoshinobu Nayatani,et al.  On exponents of a nonlinear model of chromatic adaptation , 1982 .

[9]  The conceptual NCS: An empirical investigation , 1988 .

[10]  Kenjiro Hashimoto,et al.  A nonlinear color‐appearance model using estévez‐hunt‐pointer primaries , 1987 .

[11]  Roy S. Berns A FORTRAN program for predicting the effects of chromatic adaptation on color appearance based on current CIE recommendations , 1986 .

[12]  Fred W. Billmeyer,et al.  Survey of color order systems , 1987 .

[13]  H. Helson Fundamental problems in color vision. I. The principle governing changes in hue, saturation, and lightness of non-selective samples in chromatic illumination. , 1938 .

[14]  Roy S. Berns,et al.  Methods for generating spectral reflectance functions leading to color‐constant properties , 1985 .

[15]  F. Billmeyer,et al.  Interrelation of the natural color system and the munsell color order system , 1987 .

[16]  R. Hunt,et al.  A colour‐appearance transform for the CIE 1931 standard colorimetric observer , 1985 .

[17]  R. Hunt,et al.  A model of colour vision for predicting colour appearance in various viewing conditions , 1987 .

[18]  D. Jameson,et al.  Complexities of perceived brightness. , 1961, Science.

[19]  Arne Valberg,et al.  Towards a uniform color space: A better formula to describe the munsell and OSA color scales , 1986 .

[20]  D. L. Macadam Uniform color scales. , 1974, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[21]  Y. Nayatani,et al.  Formulation of a Nonlinear Model of Chromatic Adaptation , 1981 .

[22]  Roy S. Berns,et al.  Development of the 1929 munsell book of color: A historical review , 1985 .

[23]  S S Stevens,et al.  To Honor Fechner and Repeal His Law: A power function, not a log function, describes the operating characteristic of a sensory system. , 1961, Science.

[24]  R. Hunt,et al.  The Effects of Daylight and Tungsten Light-Adaptation on Color Perception*,† , 1950 .