Testing the Tolerance Principle: Children form productive rules when it is more computationally efficient to do so

During language acquisition, children must learn when to generalize a pattern – applying it broadly and to new words (‘add –ed’ in English) – and when to restrict generalization, storing the pattern only with specific lexical items. One effort to quantify the conditions for generalization, the Tolerance Principle, has been shown to accurately predict children’s generalizations in dozens of corpus-based studies. This principle hypothesizes that a general rule will be formed when it is computationally more efficient than storing lexical forms individually. It is formalized as: a rule R will generalize if the number of exceptions does not exceed the number of words in the category N divided by the natural log of N (N/lnN). Here we test the principle in an artificial language of 9 nonsense nouns. As predicted, children exposed to 5 regular forms and 4 exceptions generalized, applying the regular form to 100% of novel test words. Children exposed to 3 regular forms and 6 exceptions did not extend the rule, even though the token frequency of the regular form was still high in this condition. The Tolerance Principle thus appears to capture a basic principle of generalization in rule formation.

[1]  J. Berko The Child's Learning of English Morphology , 1958 .

[2]  E. Newport,et al.  Getting it right by getting it wrong: When learners change languages , 2009, Cognitive Psychology.

[3]  B Commoner What price productivity. , 1976, Hospital practice.

[4]  Anna L. Theakston,et al.  A dense corpus study of past tense and plural overregularization in English. , 2004, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[5]  V. Marchman,et al.  U-shaped learning and frequency effects in a multi-layered perception: Implications for child language acquisition , 1991, Cognition.

[6]  R. Mccall,et al.  The Genetic and Environmental Origins of Learning Abilities and Disabilities in the Early School , 2007, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[7]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Rules or connections in past-tense inflections: what does the evidence rule out? , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[8]  Peter M. Duppenthaler Maturational Constraints on Language Learning , 1990 .

[9]  Shanley Allen,et al.  Aspects of Argument Structure Acquisition in Inuktitut , 1996 .

[10]  Carla L. Hudson Kam,et al.  Regularizing Unpredictable Variation: The Roles of Adult and Child Learners in Language Formation and Change , 2005 .

[11]  Linda Wilbanks,et al.  IT Productivity = ?? , 2009, IT Prof..

[12]  S. Pinker,et al.  The past and future of the past tense , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[13]  M. Penke,et al.  The Acquisition of Agreement Morphology and its Syntactic Consequences: New Evidence on German Child Language from the Simone-Corpus , 1992 .

[14]  Yvonne Freeh,et al.  Words And Rules The Ingredients Of Language , 2016 .

[15]  Katherine Demuth,et al.  The Acquisition of Bantu Languages , 2003 .

[16]  Kamil Ud Deen,et al.  The acquisition of Swahili , 2005 .

[17]  Charles Yang,et al.  For and against frequencies , 2015, Journal of Child Language.

[18]  Harald Clahsen,et al.  The development of regular and irregular verb inflection in Spanish child language , 2002, Journal of Child Language.

[19]  S Pinker,et al.  Overregularization in language acquisition. , 1992, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.