On the nature of the delayed “inhibitory” Cueing effects generated by uninformative arrows at fixation
暂无分享,去创建一个
Matthew D. Hilchey | Raymond M Klein | Jason Ivanoff | Jason Satel | Matthew D Hilchey | R. Klein | J. Ivanoff | J. Satel | Jason Ivanoff
[1] Petroc Sumner,et al. Tight coupling between positive and reversed priming in the masked prime paradigm. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[2] J. R. Simon,et al. Reactions toward the source of stimulation. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.
[3] R. Klein,et al. Visual and motor effects in inhibition of return. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[4] A. Kingstone,et al. Attention to Arrows: Pointing to a New Direction , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[5] P. Jaśkowski. The effect of nonmasking distractors on the priming of motor responses. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[6] T. Carr,et al. Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory and Language , 1994 .
[7] C. A. Marzi,et al. Distribution in the visual field of the costs of voluntarily allocated attention and of the inhibitory after-effects of covert orienting , 1987, Neuropsychologia.
[8] Martin Eimer,et al. Masked prime stimuli can bias “free” choices between response alternatives , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[9] A. Treisman,et al. A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.
[10] P. Sumner. Negative and positive masked-priming – implications for motor inhibition , 2008, Advances in cognitive psychology.
[11] Jennifer T. Coull,et al. Attention and Time , 2010 .
[12] M. Posner,et al. Components of visual orienting , 1984 .
[13] Focal spatial attention can eliminate inhibition of return , 2012, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[14] E. Lauber,et al. Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[15] Perceptual and motor inhibition of return: components or flavors? , 2012, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[16] T. L. Taylor,et al. Inhibition of return and repetition priming effects in localization and discrimination tasks. , 2005, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.
[17] P. Calabresi,et al. Saccade preparation inhibits reorienting to recently attended locations. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[18] R. Klein,et al. Inhibition of return , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[19] G. Cole,et al. What the experimenter’s prime tells the observer’s brain , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[20] Greg L. West,et al. Testing whether gaze cues and arrow cues produce reflexive or volitional shifts of attention , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[21] Alan Kingstone,et al. A new form of human spatial attention: Automated symbolic orienting , 2012 .
[22] Amy Devine,et al. Action planning in the presence of distracting stimuli: an investigation into the time course of distractor effects. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[23] Richard Ridderinkhof. Micro- and macro-adjustments of task set: activation and suppression in conflict tasks , 2002, Psychological research.
[24] J. Tipples. Eye gaze is not unique: Automatic orienting in response to uninformative arrows , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[25] J. Pratt,et al. Inhibition of return in cue–target and target–target tasks , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[26] A. Kingstone,et al. Are eyes special? It depends on how you look at it , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[27] Giovanni Berlucchi,et al. Inhibition of return: A phenomenon in search of a mechanism and a better name , 2006, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[28] Raymond M Klein,et al. Orienting of attention without awareness is affected by measurement-induced attentional control settings. , 2003, Journal of vision.
[29] R. Klein,et al. The presence of a nonresponding effector increases inhibition of return , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[30] D. Bouwhuis,et al. Attention and performance X : control of language processes , 1986 .
[31] S. Kornblum,et al. Stimulus-response compatibility with relevant and irrelevant stimulus dimensions that do and do not overlap with the response. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[32] A. Kingstone,et al. Unmasking the inhibition of return phenomenon , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.
[33] R. Klein. On the Role of Endogenous Orienting in the Inhibitory Aftermath of Exogenous Orienting , 2005 .
[34] R. Klein,et al. Categories of cognitive inhibition with reference to attention. , 1994 .
[35] Juan Lupiáñez,et al. Inhibition of return , 2010 .
[36] Raymond M. Klein,et al. Oculomotor inhibition of return , 2011 .
[37] M. Posner,et al. Inhibition of return : Neural basis and function , 1985 .
[38] Jillian H. Fecteau,et al. Correlates of Capture of Attention and Inhibition of Return across Stages of Visual Processing , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[39] Kristie R. Dukewich. Reconceptualizing inhibition of return as , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[40] Martin Eimer,et al. Motor activation with and without inhibition: Evidence for a threshold mechanism in motor control , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.
[41] J. Ivanoff,et al. Nonattentional effects of nonpredictive central cues , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[42] P. Jaśkowski,et al. How important is a prime’s gestalt for subliminal priming? , 2007, Consciousness and Cognition.