Clinicians’ Perceptions of the Benefits and Harms of Prostate and Colorectal Cancer Screening

Background. Clinicians’ perceptions of screening benefits and harms influence their recommendations, which in turn shape patients’ screening decisions. We sought to understand clinicians’ perceptions of the benefits and harms of cancer screening by comparing 2 screening tests that differ in their balance of potential benefits to harms: colonoscopy, which results in net benefit for many adults, and prostate-specific antigen (PSA) testing, which may do more harm than good. Methods. In this cross-sectional study, 126 clinicians at 24 family/internal medicine practices completed surveys in which they listed and rated the magnitude of colonoscopy and PSA testing benefits and harms for a hypothetical 70-year-old male patient and then estimated the likelihood that these tests would cause harm and lengthen the life of 100 similar men in the next 10 years. We tested the hypothesis that the availability heuristic would explain the association of screening test to perceived likelihood of benefit/harm and a competing hypothesis that clinicians’ gist of screening tests as good or bad would mediate this association. Results. Clinicians perceived PSA testing to have a greater likelihood of harm and a lower likelihood of lengthening life relative to colonoscopy. Consistent with our gist hypothesis, these associations were mediated by clinicians’ gist of screening (balance of perceived benefits to perceived harms). Limitations. Generalizability beyond academic clinicians remains to be established. Conclusions. Targeting clinicians’ gist of screening, for example through graphical displays that allow clinicians to make gist-based relative magnitude comparisons, may influence their risk perception and possibly reduce overrecommendation of screening.

[1]  Stacey L. Sheridan,et al.  Have screening harms become newsworthy? News coverage of prostate and colorectal cancer screening since the 2008 USPSTF recommendation changes , 2014, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[2]  Carmen L. Lewis,et al.  The harms of screening: a proposed taxonomy and application to lung cancer screening. , 2014, JAMA internal medicine.

[3]  Valerie F Reyna,et al.  Developmental Reversals in Risky Decision Making , 2014, Psychological science.

[4]  L. Balbuena,et al.  Colorectal cancer screening practices in Saskatchewan: Survey of family physicians. , 2013, Canadian family physician Medecin de famille canadien.

[5]  S. Chambers,et al.  The influence of family history on cognitive heuristics, risk perceptions, and prostate cancer screening behavior. , 2013, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[6]  R. Heisey,et al.  Physicians' attitudes and behaviour toward screening mammography in women 40 to 49 years of age. , 2012, Canadian family physician Medecin de famille canadien.

[7]  Ellen Peters,et al.  Decision tool to improve the quality of care in rheumatoid arthritis , 2012, Arthritis care & research.

[8]  J. Havránková Is the treatment of obesity futile?: YES. , 2012, Canadian family physician Medecin de famille canadien.

[9]  T. Perneger,et al.  Does information about risks and benefits improve the decision-making process in cancer screening - randomized study. , 2011, Cancer epidemiology.

[10]  Valerie F Reyna,et al.  Neurobiological and memory models of risky decision making in adolescents versus young adults. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[11]  A. DeGroff,et al.  A Qualitative Analysis of Lung Cancer Screening Practices by Primary Care Physicians , 2011, Journal of Community Health.

[12]  Jeffrey R. Harris,et al.  Prostate cancer screening and informed decision-making: provider and patient perspectives , 2011, Prostate Cancer and Prostatic Diseases.

[13]  T. Perneger,et al.  Impact of information about risks and benefits of cancer screening on intended participation. , 2010, European journal of cancer.

[14]  Christopher R Wolfe,et al.  Assessing semantic coherence and logical fallacies in joint probability estimates , 2010, Behavior research methods.

[15]  Carmen L. Lewis,et al.  Physicians’ Decisions About Continuing or Stopping Colon Cancer Screening in the Elderly: A Qualitative Study , 2009, Journal of General Internal Medicine.

[16]  Douglas K Rex,et al.  American College of Gastroenterology Guidelines for Colorectal Cancer Screening 2008 , 2022 .

[17]  Bernadette Mazurek Melnyk,et al.  Screening for colorectal cancer: U.S. Preventive Services Task Force recommendation statement. , 2008, Annals of internal medicine.

[18]  V. Reyna,et al.  A Theory of Medical Decision Making and Health: Fuzzy Trace Theory , 2008, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[19]  Todd E. Bodner,et al.  What Improves with Increased Missing Data Imputations? , 2008 .

[20]  Allan S Detsky,et al.  Everyone's a little bit biased (even physicians). , 2008, JAMA.

[21]  Diana Petitti,et al.  Update on the Methods of the U.S. Preventive Services Task Force: Estimating Certainty and Magnitude of Net Benefit , 2007, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[22]  C. Bernstein,et al.  Frequency of colorectal cancer screening and the impact of family physicians on screening behaviour , 2007, Canadian Medical Association Journal.

[23]  V. Reyna,et al.  Converging evidence supports fuzzy-trace theory's nested sets hypothesis, but not the frequency hypothesis , 2007 .

[24]  V. Reyna,et al.  A web exercise in evidence-based medicine using cognitive theory , 2001, Journal of General Internal Medicine.

[25]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk — Source link , 2007 .

[26]  V. Reyna,et al.  Physician decision making and cardiac risk: effects of knowledge, risk perception, risk tolerance, and fuzzy processing. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[27]  V. Reyna,et al.  Risk and Rationality in Adolescent Decision Making , 2006, Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society.

[28]  D. Faigel,et al.  ASGE guideline: colorectal cancer screening and surveillance. , 2006, Gastrointestinal endoscopy.

[29]  P. Edwards,et al.  Meta-analysis of randomised trials of monetary incentives and response to mailed questionnaires , 2005, Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health.

[30]  Valerie F. Reyna,et al.  Coherence and Correspondence Criteria for Rationality: Experts' Estimation of Risks of Sexually Transmitted Infections. , 2005 .

[31]  E. R. Peay,et al.  The Evaluation of Medical Symptoms by Patients and Doctors , 1998, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[32]  M. Mehta,et al.  Evaluation of a visual risk communication tool: effects on knowledge and perception of blood transfusion risk , 2003, Transfusion.

[33]  Valerie F Reyna,et al.  Fuzzy‐Trace Theory, Risk Communication, and Product Labeling in Sexually Transmitted Diseases , 2003, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[34]  Winston R. Sieck,et al.  Foreground:background salience: Explaining the effects of graphical displays on risk avoidance , 2003 .

[35]  V F Reyna,et al.  Genetic testing and medical decision making. , 2001, Archives of internal medicine.

[36]  J. Preisser,et al.  Factors influencing use of the prostate-specific antigen screening test in primary care. , 2000, The American journal of managed care.

[37]  J. Schafer Multiple imputation: a primer , 1999, Statistical methods in medical research.

[38]  C J McDonald,et al.  Medical Heuristics: The Silent Adjudicators of Clinical Practice , 1996, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[39]  N. Weinstein Testing four competing theories of health-protective behavior. , 1993, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[40]  C MacLeod,et al.  Memory accessibility and probability judgments: an experimental evaluation of the availability heuristic. , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[41]  V. Reyna Class inclusion, the conjunction fallacy, and other cognitive illusions , 1991 .

[42]  A. MacLeod,et al.  Worry is reasonable: the role of explanations in pessimism about future personal events. , 1991, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[43]  Roy M. Poses,et al.  Availability, Wishful Thinking, and Physicians' Diagnostic Judgments for Patients with Suspected Bacteremia , 1991, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[44]  A D Mickalide,et al.  U.S. Preventive Services Task Force. , 1986, Pediatric clinics of North America.

[45]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[46]  B. Fischhoff,et al.  Lay Foibles and Expert Fables in Judgments about Risk , 1982 .

[47]  D. Rubin,et al.  Maximum likelihood from incomplete data via the EM - algorithm plus discussions on the paper , 1977 .

[48]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.

[49]  Daniel Kahneman,et al.  Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability , 1973 .

[50]  E. Tulving,et al.  Availability versus accessibility of information in memory for words , 1966 .