Saccade Reorienting Is Facilitated by Pausing the Oculomotor Program

As we look around the world, selecting our targets, competing events may occur at other locations. Depending on current goals, the viewer must decide whether to look at new events or to ignore them. Two experimental paradigms formalize these response options: double-step saccades and saccadic inhibition. In the first, the viewer must reorient to a newly appearing target; in the second, they must ignore it. Until now, the relationship between reorienting and inhibition has been unexplored. In three experiments, we found saccadic inhibition ∼100 msec after a new target onset, regardless of the task instruction. Moreover, if this automatic inhibition is boosted by an irrelevant flash, reorienting is facilitated, suggesting that saccadic inhibition plays a crucial role in visual behavior, as a bottom–up brake that buys the time needed for decisional processes to act. Saccadic inhibition may be a ubiquitous pause signal that provides the flexibility for voluntary behavior to emerge.

[1]  R. McIntosh,et al.  Saccadic inhibition can cause the remote distractor effect, but the remote distractor effect may not be a useful concept. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[2]  Saccadic inhibition and the remote distractor effect: One mechanism or two? , 2015, Journal of vision.

[3]  J. Schall,et al.  Role of frontal eye fields in countermanding saccades: visual, movement, and fixation activity. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  Iain D Gilchrist,et al.  A population coding account for systematic variation in saccadic dead time. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  D. Munoz,et al.  Competitive Integration of Visual and Preparatory Signals in the Superior Colliculus during Saccadic Programming , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  D. Munoz,et al.  Comparison of the discharge characteristics of brain stem omnipause neurons and superior colliculus fixation neurons in monkey: implications for control of fixation and saccade behavior. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  D. P. Hanes,et al.  Controlled Movement Processing: Superior Colliculus Activity Associated with Countermanded Saccades , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  F. A. Miles,et al.  Voluntary saccadic eye movements in humans studied with a double-cue paradigm , 2002, Vision Research.

[9]  Antimo Buonocore,et al.  Modulation of saccadic inhibition by distractor size and location , 2012, Vision Research.

[10]  D. Munoz,et al.  Lateral interactions in the superior colliculus, not an extended fixation zone, can account for the remote distractor effect , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[11]  E. Reingold,et al.  Saccadic Inhibition and Gaze Contingent Research Paradigms , 2000 .

[12]  G. Logan,et al.  Inhibitory control in mind and brain: an interactive race model of countermanding saccades. , 2007, Psychological review.

[13]  G. D. Logan,et al.  Dynamics of saccade target selection: Race model analysis of double step and search step saccade production in human and macaque , 2007, Vision Research.

[14]  W. Becker,et al.  An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli , 1979, Vision Research.

[15]  Leanne Boucher,et al.  Stopping eye and hand movements: Are the processes independent? , 2007, Perception & psychophysics.

[16]  R. Walker,et al.  A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[17]  Antimo Buonocore,et al.  Saccadic inhibition underlies the remote distractor effect , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  Eyal M. Reingold,et al.  Saccadic Inhibition in Voluntary and Reflexive Saccades , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  Ziad M Hafed,et al.  A Causal Role for the Cortical Frontal Eye Fields in Microsaccade Deployment , 2016, PLoS biology.

[20]  B. Fischer,et al.  Differential effects of non-target stimuli on the occurrence of express saccades in man , 1994, Vision Research.

[21]  David Melcher,et al.  Beyond the point of no return: effects of visual distractors on saccade amplitude and velocity. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  Kitty Z. Xu,et al.  Inhibition of voluntary saccadic eye movement commands by abrupt visual onsets. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  E. Reingold,et al.  Chapter 17 – Using the Saccadic Inhibition Paradigm to Investigate Saccadic Control in Reading , 2003 .

[24]  R. W. Kentridge,et al.  Independent contributions of the orienting of attention, fixation offset and bilateral stimulation on human saccadic latencies , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  Laurence R. Harris,et al.  Small Saccades to Double-Stepped Targets Moving in Two Dimensions , 1984 .

[26]  Eyal M Reingold,et al.  Saccadic inhibition in reading. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  R. McIntosh,et al.  Attention modulates saccadic inhibition magnitude , 2013, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[28]  E. Salinas,et al.  The Countermanding Task Revisited: Fast Stimulus Detection Is a Key Determinant of Psychophysical Performance , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Patrick G. Bissett,et al.  Stop before you leap: changing eye and hand movements requires stopping. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  Daniel K. Leventhal,et al.  Canceling actions involves a race between basal ganglia pathways , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  A. Bompas,et al.  Saccadic Inhibition Reveals the Timing of Automatic and Voluntary Signals in the Human Brain , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Relation of frontal eye field activity to saccade initiation during a countermanding task , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  Eyal M. Reingold,et al.  SACCADIC INHIBITION IN COMPLEX VISUAL TASKS , 1999 .

[34]  M Missal,et al.  Common inhibitory mechanism for saccades and smooth-pursuit eye movements. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  R. Aslin,et al.  The amplitude and angle of saccades to double-step target displacements , 1987, Vision Research.

[36]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Role of the basal ganglia in the control of purposive saccadic eye movements. , 2000, Physiological reviews.

[37]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Effect of mean reaction time on saccadic responses to two-step stimuli with horizontal and vertical components , 1975, Vision Research.

[38]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neurons with complex visual properties in the superior colliculus of the macaque monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.