Shorter Lines Facilitate Reading in Those Who Struggle

People with dyslexia, who ordinarily struggle to read, sometimes remark that reading is easier when e-readers are used. Here, we used eye tracking to observe high school students with dyslexia as they read using these devices. Among the factors investigated, we found that reading using a small device resulted in substantial benefits, improving reading speeds by 27%, reducing the number of fixations by 11%, and importantly, reducing the number of regressive saccades by more than a factor of 2, with no cost to comprehension. Given that an expected trade-off between horizontal and vertical regression was not observed when line lengths were altered, we speculate that these effects occur because sluggish attention spreads perception to the left as the gaze shifts during reading. Short lines eliminate crowded text to the left, reducing regression. The effects of attention modulation by the hand, and of increased letter spacing to reduce crowding, were also found to modulate the oculomotor dynamics in reading, but whether these factors resulted in benefits or costs depended on characteristics, such as visual attention span, that varied within our sample.

[1]  K. Rayner,et al.  Extraction of information to the left of the fixated word in reading. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  Attention during sequences of saccades along marked and memorized paths , 2009, Vision Research.

[3]  Sing-Hang Cheung,et al.  The case for the visual span as a sensory bottleneck in reading. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[4]  K. Rayner Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. , 1998, Psychological bulletin.

[5]  R Hari,et al.  Prolonged attentional dwell time in dyslexic adults , 1999, Neuroscience Letters.

[6]  Stefan Gezeck,et al.  Poor saccadic control correlates with dyslexia , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  R. Abrams,et al.  Switching between global and local scopes of attention is resisted near the hands , 2012 .

[8]  J. Brockmole,et al.  The hands shield attention from visual interference , 2012, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[9]  K Rayner,et al.  Reading without a fovea. , 1979, Science.

[10]  H. BOUMA,et al.  Interaction Effects in Parafoveal Letter Recognition , 1970, Nature.

[11]  Gerhard Sonnert,et al.  Using Technology to Support STEM Reading , 2010 .

[12]  Tony Ro,et al.  Inhibition of return in saccadic eye movements , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  Yury Petrov,et al.  Asymmetries and idiosyncratic hot spots in crowding , 2011, Vision Research.

[14]  J. Henderson,et al.  Targeting regressions: Do readers pay attention to the left? , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[15]  A. Davies,et al.  Adults with dyslexia demonstrate space-based and object-based covert attention deficits: Shifting attention to the periphery and shifting attention between objects in the left visual field , 2005, Brain and Cognition.

[16]  L. Ciaccia,et al.  The Immortal Life of Henrietta Lacks , 2010, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.

[17]  G. Conti-Ramsden,et al.  The attentional blink reveals sluggish attentional shifting in adolescents with specific language impairment , 2007, Brain and Cognition.

[18]  É. Siéroff,et al.  Absence of Right Visual Field Attentional Bias in Reading in Dyslexic Children , 2011, Behavioural neurology.

[19]  K. Weaver,et al.  Relationships between the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition , 2003, Psychological reports.

[20]  J. Mitchell Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing , 2001 .

[21]  Jeff Hardin,et al.  Semaphorin and Eph Receptor Signaling Guide a Series of Cell Movements for Ventral Enclosure in C. elegans , 2012, Current Biology.

[22]  R. Remington,et al.  Eye Movement Targets Are Released from Visual Crowding , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  D. Balota,et al.  The interaction of contextual constraints and parafoveal visual information in reading , 1985, Cognitive Psychology.

[24]  Albertus A. Wijers,et al.  Dyslexics show a deviant lateralization of attentional control: a brain potential study , 2005, Neuroscience Letters.

[25]  Alexander Pollatsek,et al.  Selective attentional dyslexia , 1989 .

[26]  Pierluigi Zoccolotti,et al.  Length Effect in Word Naming in Reading: Role of Reading Experience and Reading Deficit in Italian Readers , 2005, Developmental neuropsychology.

[27]  Jefferson D. Grubb,et al.  Hands up: attentional prioritization of space near the hand. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  Feng Du,et al.  When meaning matters, look but don’t touch: The effects of posture on reading , 2010, Memory & cognition.

[29]  K. Rayner,et al.  Asymmetry of the effective visual field in reading , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.

[30]  C. Rorden,et al.  Lateralized temporal order judgement in dyslexia , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  S. Valdois,et al.  Developmental dyslexia: The visual attention span deficit hypothesis , 2007, Cognition.

[32]  Ulrich W. Weger,et al.  Attention and Eye Movements in Reading , 2006, Psychological science.

[33]  Renske de Leeuw,et al.  Special font for dyslexia , 2010 .

[34]  Daniel Zwillinger,et al.  CRC Standard Probability and Statistics Tables and Formulae, Student Edition , 1999 .

[35]  H. Bouma,et al.  Foveal and parafoveal recognition of letters and words by dyslexics and by average readers , 1977, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  John O. Willis,et al.  Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Third Edition , 2008 .

[37]  Rita G. Rudel,et al.  Naming of object-drawings by dyslexic and other learning disabled children , 1976, Brain and Language.

[38]  Tracey D. Berger,et al.  Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[39]  Alexander Pollatsek,et al.  Asymmetries in the perceptual span for Israeli readers , 1981, Brain and Language.

[40]  B. Shaywitz,et al.  Dyslexia (Specific Reading Disability) , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.

[41]  Denis Cousineau,et al.  Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson's method , 2005 .

[42]  Joseph K. Torgesen,et al.  Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing , 1997 .

[43]  M. Molteni,et al.  Visual and auditory attentional capture are both sluggish in children with developmental dyslexia. , 2005, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[44]  Benjamin R. Dering,et al.  Behavioral and ERP evidence for amodal sluggish attentional shifting in developmental dyslexia , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[45]  H. Renvall,et al.  Left minineglect in dyslexic adults. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[46]  J. Talcott,et al.  Adults with dyslexia exhibit large effects of crowding, increased dependence on cues, and detrimental effects of distractors in visual search tasks , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  J. Lettvin,et al.  Peripheral vision in persons with dyslexia. , 1987, The New England journal of medicine.

[48]  Albrecht W. Inhoff,et al.  Attention allocation to the right and left of a fixated word: Use of orthographic information from multiple words during reading , 2004 .

[49]  J. Ziegler,et al.  Extra-large letter spacing improves reading in dyslexia , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[50]  J. Brockmole,et al.  A bias to detail: how hand position modulates visual learning and visual memory , 2012, Memory & cognition.

[51]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  Sluggish engagement and disengagement of non-spatial attention in dyslexic children , 2008, Cortex.

[52]  D. Spinelli,et al.  Crowding, reading, and developmental dyslexia. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[53]  D. Spinelli,et al.  Crowding Effects on Word Identification in Developmental Dyslexia , 2002, Cortex.

[54]  Yury Petrov,et al.  Locus of spatial attention determines inward-outward anisotropy in crowding. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[55]  E. Marinus,et al.  Australian comparison data for the Test of Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE) , 2013 .

[56]  M. Molteni,et al.  The gradient of visual attention in developmental dyslexia , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[57]  Left minineglect or inverse pseudoneglect in children withdyslexia? , 2011, Neuroreport.

[58]  H. Renvall,et al.  Impaired processing of rapid stimulus sequences in dyslexia , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[59]  J. M. Cattell,et al.  The Psychological Corporation , 1923, Science.

[60]  S. Gori,et al.  Action Video Games Make Dyslexic Children Read Better , 2013, Current Biology.

[61]  K. Amunts,et al.  Cognitive subtypes of dyslexia. , 2008, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[62]  M. Turatto,et al.  Orienting of visual attention in dyslexia: evidence for asymmetric hemispheric control of attention , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[63]  M. Turatto,et al.  Visual-Spatial Attention in Developmental Dyslexia , 2000, Cortex.

[64]  G. Legge,et al.  The effect of print size on reading speed in dyslexia. , 2005, Journal of research in reading.

[65]  F. Ramus,et al.  What Phonological Deficit? , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[66]  David Beymer,et al.  Wide vs. Narrow Paragraphs: An Eye Tracking Analysis , 2005, INTERACT.

[67]  M. Turatto,et al.  Asymmetrical visual fields distribution of attention in dyslexic children: a neuropsychological study , 2000, Neuroscience Letters.

[68]  Miles A. Tinker,et al.  Influence of line width on eye movements , 1940 .

[69]  A. Collins The Psychology of Memory. , 2001 .

[70]  S. Gori,et al.  A Causal Link between Visual Spatial Attention and Reading Acquisition , 2012, Current Biology.

[71]  H. Blanchard,et al.  The acquisition of parafoveal word information in reading , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[72]  S. Valdois,et al.  The visual attention span deficit in dyslexia is visual and not verbal , 2012, Cortex.

[73]  W. Beyer CRC Standard Probability And Statistics Tables and Formulae , 1990 .