Nonlinear analysis of irregular animal vocalizations.

Animal vocalizations range from almost periodic vocal-fold vibration to completely atonal turbulent noise. Between these two extremes, a variety of nonlinear dynamics such as limit cycles, subharmonics, biphonation, and chaotic episodes have been recently observed. These observations imply possible functional roles of nonlinear dynamics in animal acoustic communication. Nonlinear dynamics may also provide insight into the degree to which detailed features of vocalizations are under close neural control, as opposed to more directly reflecting biomechanical properties of the vibrating vocal folds themselves. So far, nonlinear dynamical structures of animal voices have been mainly studied with spectrograms. In this study, the deterministic versus stochastic (DVS) prediction technique was used to quantify the amount of nonlinearity in three animal vocalizations: macaque screams, piglet screams, and dog barks. Results showed that in vocalizations with pronounced harmonic components (adult macaque screams, certain piglet screams, and dog barks), deterministic nonlinear prediction was clearly more powerful than stochastic linear prediction. The difference, termed low-dimensional nonlinearity measure (LNM), indicates the presence of a low-dimensional attractor. In highly irregular signals such as juvenile macaque screams, piglet screams, and some dog barks, the detectable amount of nonlinearity was comparatively small. Analyzing 120 samples of dog barks, it was further shown that the harmonic-to-noise ratio (HNR) was positively correlated with LNM. It is concluded that nonlinear analysis is primarily useful in animal vocalizations with strong harmonic components (including subharmonics and biphonation) or low-dimensional chaos.

[1]  D. U. Feddersen-Petersen,et al.  Vocalization of European wolves ( Canis lupus lupus L.) and various dog breeds ( Canis lupus f. fam.) , 2000 .

[2]  N. Fletcher A class of chaotic bird calls? , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[3]  David Ruelle,et al.  Deterministic chaos: the science and the fiction , 1995 .

[4]  A. Behrman,et al.  Microphone and electroglottographic data from dysphonic patients: type 1, 2 and 3 signals. , 1998, Journal of voice : official journal of the Voice Foundation.

[5]  I. Devore,et al.  Baboon Social Behavior , 1965 .

[6]  S. K. Mullick,et al.  NONLINEAR DYNAMICAL ANALYSIS OF SPEECH , 1996 .

[7]  Theiler,et al.  Spurious dimension from correlation algorithms applied to limited time-series data. , 1986, Physical review. A, General physics.

[8]  Günter Tembrock Canid vocalizations , 1976, Behavioural Processes.

[9]  Michael J. Owren,et al.  Some Analysis Methods That May be Useful to Acoustic Primatologists , 1995 .

[10]  H. Schuckman Primate Behavior: Field Studies of Monkeys and Apes. , 1966 .

[11]  Albano,et al.  Filtered noise can mimic low-dimensional chaotic attractors. , 1993, Physical review. E, Statistical physics, plasmas, fluids, and related interdisciplinary topics.

[12]  W Seidner,et al.  Nonlinear phenomena in the natural howling of a dog-wolf mix. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[13]  D. Berry,et al.  Bifurcations in excised larynx experiments. , 1995, Journal of voice : official journal of the Voice Foundation.

[14]  B. Townshend,et al.  Nonlinear prediction of speech , 1991, [Proceedings] ICASSP 91: 1991 International Conference on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing.

[15]  G. Manteuffel,et al.  COMMON FEATURES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN NURSE GRUNTING OF DOMESTIC PIGS (SUS SCROFA): A MULTI-PARAMETRIC ANALYSIS , 1999 .

[16]  Farmer,et al.  Predicting chaotic time series. , 1987, Physical review letters.

[17]  M. Hauser The Evolution of Nonhuman Primate Vocalizations: Effects of Phylogeny, Body Weight, and Social Context , 1993, The American Naturalist.

[18]  Bijan Pesaran,et al.  The role of nonlinear dynamics of the syrinx in the vocalizations of a songbird , 1998, Nature.

[19]  John E. Markel,et al.  Linear Prediction of Speech , 1976, Communication and Cybernetics.

[20]  H. Herzel,et al.  The harmonic-to-noise ratio applied to dog barks. , 2001, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[21]  A. Alwan,et al.  A nonlinear dynamical systems analysis of fricative consonants. , 1995, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[22]  H. Herzel,et al.  Bifurcations in an asymmetric vocal-fold model. , 1995, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[23]  Charles H. Brown Current Topics in Primate Vocal Communication , 1997 .

[24]  David Ruelle,et al.  Where Can One Hope to Profitably Apply the Ideas of Chaos , 1994 .

[25]  Charles H. Brown,et al.  Modes of vocal variation in Sykes's monkey (Cercopithecus albogularis) squeals. , 1995, Journal of comparative psychology.

[26]  Charles H. Brown,et al.  Laryngeal biomechanics and vocal communication in the squirrel monkey (Saimiri boliviensis). , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[27]  Leonard A. Smith Intrinsic limits on dimension calculations , 1988 .

[28]  D. Berry,et al.  Analysis of vocal disorders with methods from nonlinear dynamics. , 1994, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[29]  James Theiler,et al.  Constrained-realization Monte-carlo Method for Hypothesis Testing , 1996 .

[30]  Ulrich Parlitz,et al.  Methods of chaos physics and their application to acoustics , 1988 .

[31]  D. Ruelle,et al.  The Claude Bernard Lecture, 1989 - Deterministic chaos: the science and the fiction , 1990, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. A. Mathematical and Physical Sciences.

[32]  Hanspeter Herzel,et al.  Calls out of chaos: the adaptive significance of nonlinear phenomena in mammalian vocal production , 2002, Animal Behaviour.

[33]  Jack W. Bradbury,et al.  Principles of Animal Communication , 1998 .

[34]  D. Berry,et al.  Interpretation of biomechanical simulations of normal and chaotic vocal fold oscillations with empirical eigenfunctions. , 1994, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[35]  S N Awan,et al.  Improvements in estimating the harmonics-to-noise ratio of the voice. , 1994, Journal of voice : official journal of the Voice Foundation.

[36]  H. Herzel,et al.  SUBHARMONICS, BIPHONATION, AND DETERMINISTIC CHAOS IN MAMMAL VOCALIZATION , 1998 .

[37]  F. Takens Detecting strange attractors in turbulence , 1981 .

[38]  Thomas Murry,et al.  Infant Communication: Cry and Early Speech , 1980 .

[39]  H. Kantz,et al.  Nonlinear time series analysis , 1997 .

[40]  Rm. Schassburger,et al.  Vocal communication in the timber wolf, Canis lupus, Linnaeus. Structure, motivation, and ontogeny , 1993 .

[41]  T. Baer,et al.  Harmonics-to-noise ratio as an index of the degree of hoarseness. , 1982, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[42]  H. Gouzoules,et al.  Agonistic screams differ among four species of macaques: the significance of motivation-structural rules , 2000, Animal Behaviour.

[43]  M. Casdagli Chaos and Deterministic Versus Stochastic Non‐Linear Modelling , 1992 .

[44]  Hanspeter Herzel,et al.  Bifurcations and chaos in newborn infant cries , 1990 .

[45]  James Theiler,et al.  Testing for nonlinearity in time series: the method of surrogate data , 1992 .

[46]  Schreiber,et al.  Improved Surrogate Data for Nonlinearity Tests. , 1996, Physical review letters.

[47]  Robert M. May,et al.  Simple mathematical models with very complicated dynamics , 1976, Nature.

[48]  Lars Schrader,et al.  COMPUTER-AIDED ANALYSIS OF ACOUSTIC PARAMETERS IN ANIMAL VOCALISATIONS: A MULTI-PARAMETRIC APPROACH , 1997 .

[49]  Hanspeter Herzel,et al.  Modeling the role of nonhuman vocal membranes in phonation. , 1999 .

[50]  S. Green,et al.  Variation of Vocal Pattern with Social Situation in the Japanese Monkey (Macaca fuscata): A Field Study * , 1975 .

[51]  W. Fitch,et al.  Modeling the role of nonhuman vocal membranes in phonation. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[52]  A. Behrman,et al.  Global and local dimensions of vocal dynamics. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.